Perspective taking in referring to objects behind versus in front of an observer: Frames of reference, intraindividual consistency, and response latencies

Speakers of English and German typically adopt the reflection variant of the relative frame of reference (FoR) in order to describe how nonoriented objects that are located in front of them are related to one another. Little is known, however, about how they proceed in dorsal settings, with objects located in their back. In this article, we explore the turn hypothesis, which assumes a (mental) 180° turn of the observer to face the objects, converting the dorsal into a frontal situation, so that the preferred FoR variant for frontal settings can be applied. To elicit spatial references, we used photographs that showed an observer and two objects either in the observer's visual field (frontal condition) or in the observer's back (dorsal condition). The observer was looking either in the same direction as the referencing individual (aligned perspectives) or in the opposite direction (vis-à-vis perspective). Data from two experiments show that while participants do adopt the observer's perspective, their references in dorsal settings are incompatible with the turn hypothesis. Analyses of response latencies indicate additional cognitive costs for establishing a FoR for the very first item in the dorsal condition as compared to the frontal condition, but fast adaption for subsequent items, and high intraindividual consistency in FoR choice in both conditions. Maintaining the assumption that references in dorsal settings should be compatible with the variant of the relative FoR adopted in frontal settings, participants’ references can be explained by assuming a backward projection that gets by without a (mental) turn of the observer.

[1]  Rachel A Robbins,et al.  A Review and Clarification of the Terms “holistic,” “configural,” and “relational” in the Face Perception Literature , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[2]  B. Tversky,et al.  Embodied and disembodied cognition: Spatial perspective-taking , 2009, Cognition.

[3]  B. Keysar,et al.  The Effect of Culture on Perspective Taking , 2007, Psychological science.

[4]  Clifford Alden Hill,et al.  Linguistic Representation of Spatial and Temporal Orientation , 1978 .

[5]  G. Miller,et al.  Language and Perception , 1976 .

[6]  S. Levinson,et al.  Can language restructure cognition? The case for space , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[7]  Sieghard Beller,et al.  Moving Forward in Space and Time: How Strong is the Conceptual Link between Spatial and Temporal Frames of Reference? , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[8]  Katrin Johannsen,et al.  The role of scene type and priming in the processing and selection of a spatial frame of reference , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[9]  Giovanni Bennardo,et al.  Language and space in Tonga : The front of the house is where the chief sits! , 2000 .

[10]  H. H. Clark SPACE, TIME, SEMANTICS, AND THE CHILD , 1973 .

[11]  Stephen C. Levinson,et al.  Tracking Down Abstract Linguistic Meaning: Neural Correlates of Spatial Frame of Reference Ambiguities in Language , 2012, PloS one.

[12]  Vincent Walsh A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[13]  Joachim Grabowski,et al.  A uniform anthropomorphological approach to the human conception of dimensional relations , 1999, Spatial Cogn. Comput..

[14]  Gabriela Pérez Báez,et al.  Spatial frames of reference in Mesoamerican languages , 2011 .

[15]  Paul U. Lee,et al.  Why do speakers mix perspectives? , 1999, Spatial Cogn. Comput..

[16]  Andrea Bender,et al.  Temporal frames of reference in three Germanic languages: Individual consistency, interindividual consensus, and cross-linguistic variability , 2015, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[17]  L. Talmy Toward a Cognitive Semantics , 2003 .

[18]  Clifford Hill,et al.  Up/down, front/back, left/right. A contrastive study of Hausa and English , 1982 .

[19]  M. Sheelagh T. Carpendale,et al.  Analyzing Qualitative Data , 2017, ISS.

[20]  Leonie Kohl,et al.  Mental Images And Their Transformations , 2016 .

[21]  J. Grabowski,et al.  Determinanten der Interpretation dimensionaler Lokalisationsäusserungen : Experimente in fünf Sprachen , 1996 .

[22]  Carolyn O’Meara,et al.  Vectors and frames of reference: Evidence from Seri and Yucatec , 2012 .

[23]  Henrik Singmann,et al.  Turn around to have a look? Spatial referencing in dorsal vs. frontal settings in cross-linguistic comparison , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[24]  Andrea Bender,et al.  Temporal Frames of Reference: Conceptual Analysis and Empirical Evidence from German, English, Mandarin Chinese and Tongan , 2010 .

[25]  The Role of Front-Back Features in Children's "Front,""Back," and "Beside" Placements of Objects. , 1970 .

[26]  Rick Dale,et al.  Listeners invest in an assumed other’s perspective despite cognitive cost , 2011, Cognition.

[27]  Philip R. Cohen,et al.  Referring as a Collaborative Process , 2003 .

[28]  Michael F. Schober,et al.  Speakers, addressees, and frames of reference: Whose effort is minimized in conversations about locations? , 1995 .

[29]  G. G. Abkarian Comprehension of deictic locatives: The object “Behind” it , 1982 .

[30]  Indra Sinka,et al.  The New Reynell Developmental Language Scales , 2011 .

[31]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  Space, time, and number: a Kantian research program , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[32]  Thora Tenbrink,et al.  Spatial Strategies in the Description of Complex Configurations , 2011 .

[33]  George A. Miller,et al.  Factors Affecting the Use of Dimensional Prepositions in German and American English: Object Orientation, Social Context, and Prepositional Pattern , 2000 .

[34]  Rafael E. Núñez,et al.  With the Future Behind Them: Convergent Evidence From Aymara Language and Gesture in the Crosslinguistic Comparison of Spatial Construals of Time , 2006, Cogn. Sci..

[35]  Yvon Keromnes,et al.  Space in Language and Cognition , 2007 .

[36]  Sarah Brown-Schmidt,et al.  Influence of perspective and goals on reference production in conversation , 2012, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[37]  Andrea Bender,et al.  Mapping spatial frames of reference onto time: A review of theoretical accounts and empirical findings , 2014, Cognition.

[38]  R. Meijer,et al.  A trial studying approach to predict college achievement , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[39]  David Clark-Carter,et al.  Quantitative psychological research : a student's handbook , 2004 .

[40]  M. Cox Interpretation of the Spatial Prepositions 'in front of' and 'behind' , 1981 .

[41]  Graham R. Gibbs,et al.  Analyzing Qualitative Data , 2014 .

[42]  Giovanni Bennardo,et al.  Language, Space, and Social Relationships: A Foundational Cultural Model in Polynesia , 2009 .

[43]  Kensy Cooperrider,et al.  The tangle of space and time in human cognition , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[44]  Laura A. Carlson-Radvansky,et al.  The Influence of Functional Relations on Spatial Term Selection , 1996 .