Effect of visuo-manual configuration on a telerobot integration into the body schema
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Wa Wijnand IJsselsteijn,et al. Self-attribution and telepresence , 2007 .
[2] D. Proffitt,et al. Action-specific influences on distance perception: a role for motor simulation. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[3] Jon Driver,et al. Reaching with a tool extends visual–tactile interactions into far space: evidence from cross-modal extinction , 2001, Neuropsychologia.
[4] W. Epstein,et al. Perceiving Distance: A Role of Effort and Intent , 2004, Perception.
[5] Nathaniel I. Durlach,et al. Telepresence, time delay and adaptation , 1991 .
[6] Jody C. Culham,et al. fMRI reveals a preference for near viewing in the human parieto-occipital cortex , 2007, NeuroImage.
[7] C. Gross,et al. The representation of extrapersonal space: A possible role for bimodal, visual-tactile neurons , 1995 .
[8] Atsushi Iriki,et al. Self-images in the video monitor coded by monkey intraparietal neurons , 2001, Neuroscience Research.
[9] Edoardo Bisiach,et al. The Spatial Features of Unilateral Neglect , 1997 .
[10] Jon Driver,et al. Attentional competition between modalities: extinction between touch and vision after right hemisphere damage , 1997, Neuropsychologia.
[11] M. Jeannerod. The cognitive neuroscience of action , 1997, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[12] S. Aglioti,et al. Disownership of left hand and objects related to it in a patient with right brain damage , 1996, Neuroreport.
[13] E. Bizzi,et al. The Cognitive Neurosciences , 1996 .
[14] G. Vallar. Spatial hemineglect in humans , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[15] Jeanine K. Stefanucci,et al. The Role of Effort in Perceiving Distance , 2003, Psychological science.
[16] A. Berti,et al. When Far Becomes Near: Remapping of Space by Tool Use , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[17] Yann Coello,et al. Motor representations and the perception of space: perceptual judgments of the boundary of action space , 2010 .
[18] Inaki Maurtua. Human Machine Interaction - Getting Closer , 2012 .
[19] J P Orliaguet,et al. Visuokinesthetic Realignment in a Video-Controlled Reaching Task , 2003, Journal of motor behavior.
[20] Christopher Kennard,et al. Visual neglect associated with frontal lobe infarction , 1996, Journal of Neurology.
[21] Mirko Farina. Perception, action, and consciousness: Sensorimotor Dynamics and Two Visual Systems , 2010 .
[22] Yann Coello,et al. Frame of reference and adaptation to directional bias in a video-controlled reaching task , 2002, Ergonomics.
[23] M. Tanaka,et al. Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. , 1996, Neuroreport.
[24] W. Warren,et al. Visual guidance of walking through apertures: body-scaled information for affordances. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[25] C. Spence,et al. Visual Capture of Touch: Out-of-the-Body Experiences With Rubber Gloves , 2000, Psychological science.
[26] W. Epstein,et al. Tool use affects perceived distance, but only when you intend to use it. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[27] Alice C. Roy,et al. Tool-use induces morphological updating of the body schema , 2009, Current Biology.
[28] Elisabetta Làdavas,et al. Seeing where your hands are , 1997, Nature.
[29] Emilie M. Roth,et al. Symbolic AI computer simulations as tools for investigating the dynamics of joint cognitive systems , 1995 .
[30] A. Maravita,et al. Tools for the body (schema) , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[31] J. Paillard. Brain and space , 1991 .
[32] J. Gibson. The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .
[33] John C. Marshall,et al. Spatial neglect : position papers on theory and practice , 1994 .
[34] G Rizzolatti,et al. The Space Around Us , 1997, Science.
[35] Jack M. Loomis,et al. Distal Attribution and Presence , 1992, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.
[36] G. Lakoff,et al. The Brain's concepts: the role of the Sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge , 2005, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[37] P. Haggard,et al. The rubber hand illusion revisited: visuotactile integration and self-attribution. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[38] Jens Rasmussen,et al. Cognitive Systems Engineering , 2022 .
[39] D. Perani,et al. The anatomy of unilateral neglect after right-hemisphere stroke lesions. A clinical/CT-scan correlation study in man , 1986, Neuropsychologia.
[40] A. Cowey,et al. No abrupt change in visual hemineglect from near to far space , 1998, Neuropsychologia.
[41] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Mirror Neurons Differentially Encode the Peripersonal and Extrapersonal Space of Monkeys , 2009, Science.
[42] A. Borghi,et al. Embodied cognition and beyond: Acting and sensing the body , 2010, Neuropsychologia.
[43] D. Lloyd. Spatial limits on referred touch to an alien limb may reflect boundaries of visuo-tactile peripersonal space surrounding the hand , 2007, Brain and Cognition.
[44] Daniel Mestre,et al. Implémentation de mécanismes d'anticipation visuo-motrice en téléopération , 2004 .
[45] C. Spence,et al. Attention and the crossmodal construction of space , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[46] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see , 1998, Nature.
[47] F. Pavani,et al. Left tactile extinction following visual stimulation of a rubber hand. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[48] Erik Hollnagel,et al. Cognitive Systems Engineering: New Wine in New Bottles , 1983, Int. J. Man Mach. Stud..
[49] G. Knoblich,et al. The case for motor involvement in perceiving conspecifics. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.
[50] J. Marshall,et al. Left neglect for near but not far space in man , 1991, Nature.
[51] Jason B Mattingley,et al. Effects of prismatic adaptation on judgements of spatial extent in peripersonal and extrapersonal space , 2003, Neuropsychologia.
[52] Jody C Culham,et al. Is That within Reach? fMRI Reveals That the Human Superior Parieto-Occipital Cortex Encodes Objects Reachable by the Hand , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[53] A. Noë,et al. A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.
[54] W. Prinz,et al. Perceptual resonance: action-induced modulation of perception , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[55] P. B. Pufall,et al. Perceiving Whether or Not the World Affords Stepping Onto and Over: A Developmental Study , 1992 .
[56] A. Cowey,et al. Left visuo-spatial neglect can be worse in far than in near space , 1994, Neuropsychologia.
[57] Yann Coello,et al. Embodiment, spatial categorisation and action , 2007, Consciousness and Cognition.
[58] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Coding of peripersonal space in inferior premotor cortex (area F4). , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.
[59] Fred H. Previc,et al. The Neuropsychology of 3-D Space , 1998 .
[60] Giuseppe di Pellegrino,et al. Neuropsychological Evidence of an Integrated Visuotactile Representation of Peripersonal Space in Humans , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[61] A Farnè,et al. Dynamic size‐change of hand peripersonal space following tool use , 2000, Neuroreport.
[62] M S Graziano,et al. Coding the location of the arm by sight. , 2000, Science.
[63] Jacques Paillard,et al. The hand and the tool: the functional architecture of human technical skills , 1993 .
[64] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.