Pseudohomophone effects in lexical decision: still a challenge for current word recognition models.

Computational models that implement a serial mechanism of phonological assembly predict interactions between the size of the pseudohomophone (PsH) effect and stimulus length. Models with frequency-sensitive word representations predict baseword frequency effects. These predictions were tested in a lexical-decision task. The results showed constant PsH effects across different word lengths (in favor of parallel phonological activation) and baseword frequency effects (in favor of frequency-sensitive representations). However, the baseword frequency effect was opposite of what the models predicted. This result is most easily accommodated by models that assume an orthographic verification mechanism. The plausibility of such a mechanism was further supported by the results of 2 additional experiments investigating the effects of response speed and spelling probability (feedback consistency) on the size of the PsH effect.

[1]  G. Stone,et al.  Perception Is a Two-Way Street: Feedforward and Feedback Phonology in Visual Word Recognition ☆ , 1997 .

[2]  M Coltheart,et al.  Children’s use of phonological encoding when reading for meaning , 1980, Memory & cognition.

[3]  J. Ziegler,et al.  Phonology can help or hurt the perception of print. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[4]  C. A. Becker Allocation of attention during visual word recognition. , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[5]  Conrad Perry,et al.  The DRC model of visual word recognition and reading aloud: An extension to German , 2000 .

[6]  C. Perfetti,et al.  A rose is a REEZ: The two-cycles model of phonology assembly in reading English , 1995 .

[7]  M Coltheart,et al.  DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. , 2001, Psychological review.

[8]  Gregory O. Stone,et al.  Chapter 14 “Assembled” Phonology and Reading: A Case Study in How Theoretical Perspective Shapes Empirical Investigation , 1992 .

[9]  G. C. Orden,et al.  Complex Dynamic Systems also Predict Dissociations, but They Do Not Reduce to Autonomous Components , 1997 .

[10]  G. C. Orden A ROWS is a ROSE: Spelling, sound, and reading , 1987 .

[11]  Arthur M. Jacobs,et al.  What is the pronunciation for -ough and the spelling for /u/? A database for computing feedforward and feedback consistency in English , 1997 .

[12]  Veronika Coltheart,et al.  When a ROWS is a ROSE: Phonological Effects in Written Word Comprehension , 1994 .

[13]  J. LeFevre,et al.  Base word Frequency and Pseudohomophone Naming , 1996 .

[14]  L Nyström,et al.  Statistical Analysis , 2008, Encyclopedia of Social Network Analysis and Mining.

[15]  R W Schvaneveldt,et al.  An activation--verification model for letter and word recognition: the word-superiority effect. , 1982, Psychological review.

[16]  M Taft,et al.  An alternative to grapheme-phoneme conversion rules? , 1982, Memory & cognition.

[17]  G. Stone,et al.  Strategic control of processing in word recognition. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  G. C. Orden,et al.  Interdependence of form and function in cognitive systems explains perception of printed words. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[19]  Derek Besner,et al.  Word recognition and identification: Do word-frequency effects reflect lexical access? , 1988 .

[20]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. , 1996, Psychological review.

[21]  G. E. MacKinnon,et al.  Reading Research Advances in Theory and Practice , 1985 .

[22]  D. Besner,et al.  Reading pseudohomophones: Implications for models of pronunciation assembly and the locus of word-frequency effects in naming. , 1987 .

[23]  A. Jacobs,et al.  Optimal viewing position effect in word recognition: A challenge to current theory. , 1992 .

[24]  Eckart Scheerer,et al.  Visual word recognition in German. , 1987 .

[25]  Bruce F. Pennington,et al.  Word identification in reading and the promise of subsymbolic psycholinguistics. , 1990, Psychological review.

[26]  Max Coltheart,et al.  Access to the internal lexicon , 1977 .

[27]  D. Besner,et al.  Suedohomofoan effects in visual word recognition: evidence for phonological processing. , 1983, Canadian journal of psychology.

[28]  D. A. Allport,et al.  Language perception and production : relationships between listening, speaking, reading and writing , 1987 .

[29]  H. Rubenstein,et al.  Evidence for phonemic recoding in visual word recognition , 1971 .

[30]  Paul W. B. Atkins,et al.  Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches. , 1993 .

[31]  Ken N. Seergobin,et al.  On the association between connectionism and data: Are a few words necessary? , 1990 .

[32]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Basic processes in reading : visual word recognition , 1993 .

[33]  A. Jacobs,et al.  Localist connectionist approaches to human cognition , 1998 .

[34]  G. Underwood,et al.  Strategical invariance in lexical access: The reappearance of the pseudohomophone effect. , 1988 .

[35]  Randi C. Martin,et al.  The Pseudohomophone Effect: The Role of Visual Similarity in Non-Word Decisions , 1982, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[36]  Rose-Marie Weber,et al.  A Linguistic Analysis of First-Grade Reading Errors. , 1970 .

[37]  Pseudohomophone effects and models of word recognition. , 1996 .

[38]  G. Stone,et al.  Word identification in reading and the promise of subsymbolic psycholinguistics. , 1990, Psychological review.

[39]  Roger W. Schvaneveldt,et al.  Frequency and pronounceability in visually presented naming and lexical decision tasks. , 1987 .

[40]  R. Frost Toward a strong phonological theory of visual word recognition: true issues and false trails. , 1998 .

[41]  M. Glanzer,et al.  List composition effects in lexical decision and recognition memory , 1988 .

[42]  James L. McClelland,et al.  An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. , 1981 .

[43]  J Grainger,et al.  Orthographic processing in visual word recognition: a multiple read-out model. , 1996, Psychological review.

[44]  J. Ziegler,et al.  The Feedback Consistency Effect in Lexical Decision and Naming , 1997 .

[45]  James L. McClelland,et al.  An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: part 1.: an account of basic findings , 1988 .

[46]  S. Lupker,et al.  Ambiguity and visual word recognition: can feedback explain both homophone and polysemy effects? , 1999, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[47]  Arthur M. Jacobs,et al.  Statistical analysis of the bidirectional inconsistency of spelling and sound in French , 1996 .

[48]  M. Taft,et al.  Lack of Phonological Mediation in a Semantic Categorization Task , 1998 .

[49]  B. Weekes Differential Effects of Number of Letters on Word and Nonword Naming Latency , 1997 .

[50]  Ram Frost,et al.  Orthography, phonology, morphology, and meaning , 1992 .

[51]  R. H. Baayen,et al.  The CELEX Lexical Database (CD-ROM) , 1996 .

[52]  Marcus Taft,et al.  Reading and the Mental Lexicon , 1991 .

[53]  Marcus Taft,et al.  Pseudohomophone Naming and the Word Frequency Effect , 1992 .

[54]  Jonathan Baron,et al.  Phonemic Stage Not Necessary for Reading , 1973 .

[55]  J. Ziegler,et al.  Phonological Information Provides Early Sources of Constraint in the Processing of Letter Strings , 1995 .

[56]  B. Bergum,et al.  Attention and Performance VI , 1978 .

[57]  M. Masson,et al.  Frequency effects and lexical access: On the interpretation of null pseudohomophone base-word frequency effects. , 1999 .

[58]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Effects of baseword frequency and orthographic neighborhood size in pseudohomophone naming , 2000 .

[59]  M. Coltheart,et al.  Serial processing in reading aloud: Evidence for dual-route models of reading. , 1994 .

[60]  Kenneth R. Paap,et al.  The case of the vanishing frequency effect: A retest of the verification model. , 1994 .

[61]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. , 1989, Psychological review.