On the Externalization of Auditory Images

An acoustic image is said to be 'externalized' if the sound source appears to the listener to lie outside the head. The purpose of the note is to put the phenomenon of externalization of auditory images in proper perspective. It points out that the listener's pinnae (the so-called 'pinna factor') is only one of many factors that determine the extent to which a sound image is externalized and that the relative importance of these factors is not yet clear. The note begins with some general comments on externalization. It then considers the effect of reverberation. It examines head movements, visual effects, and other factors. It concludes with remarks on externalization's relation to head-related transfer functions (HRTFs), the pinna factor, and improved experimental instrumentation.

[1]  H. Wallach,et al.  The role of head movements and vestibular and visual cues in sound localization. , 1940 .

[2]  N I Durlach,et al.  Intensity perception. I. Preliminary theory of intensity resolution. , 1969, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[3]  F E Toole,et al.  In-head localization of acoustic images. , 1970, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[4]  N Durlach Auditory Localization in Teleoperator and Virtual Environment Systems: Ideas, Issues, and Problems , 1991, Perception.

[5]  Heinrich Hertz,et al.  On the differences between localization and lateralization. , 1974, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[6]  D. Mershon,et al.  Intensity and reverberation as factors in the auditory perception of egocentric distance , 1975 .

[7]  Y. Shao,et al.  Sound Localization Cues for a Magnified Head: Implications from Sound Diffraction about a Rigid Sphere , 1993, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[8]  F L Wightman,et al.  Headphone simulation of free-field listening. II: Psychophysical validation. , 1989, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[9]  Durand R. Begault Perceptual Effects of Synthetic Reverberation on 3-D Audio Systems , 1991 .

[10]  Jack M. Loomis,et al.  Distal Attribution and Presence , 1992, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[11]  Elizabeth M. Wenzel,et al.  Localization in Virtual Acoustic Displays , 1992, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[12]  P. Young,et al.  The r?le of head movements in auditory localization. , 1931 .

[13]  D R Perrott,et al.  Discrimination of the spatial distribution of concurrently active sound sources: some experiments with stereophonic arrays. , 1984, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[14]  D. Perrott Concurrent minimum audible angle: a re-examination of the concept of auditory spatial acuity. , 1984, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[15]  H S Colburn,et al.  Model for auditory localization. , 1976, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[16]  J M Loomis,et al.  Active localization of virtual sounds. , 1990, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[17]  N I Durlach,et al.  Range effects in the identification of lateral position. , 1989, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[18]  R. Häusler,et al.  Sound localization in subjects with impaired hearing. Spatial-discrimination and interaural-discrimination tests. , 1983, Acta oto-laryngologica. Supplementum.

[19]  A. Mills On the minimum audible angle , 1958 .