Oculomotor adaptation to wedge prisms with no part of the body seen

When S looks at a visual target through prisms, adaptive shifts in reaching behavior occur even though he sees no part of his body through the prisms. These shifts are caused by a change in the judgment of the direction of gaze (oculomotor change), which in turn is caused by two secondary prismatic effects: (a) asymmetry of the visual display and (b) apparent rotation about a vertical axis of a panel or wall facing S. The “asymmetry” factor contributes 22% of the total oculomotor change, and the “rotation” effect contributes the remaining 78%. Oculomotor change is not facilitated by eye-movzment activity. The adaptive oculomotor change induces a non-adaptive proprioception change about one-tenth as large as the oculomotor change. These findings are capable of accounting for the previously unexplained results reported by Wooster in 1923, and also for the current controversy about the role of reafferent stimulation in sensorymotor adaptation.

[1]  C R HAMILTON,et al.  INTERMANUAL TRANSFER OF ADAPTATION TO PRISMS. , 1964, The American journal of psychology.

[2]  James G. Taylor The behavioral basis of perception , 1962 .

[3]  John L. Bower,et al.  Selective intermanual transfer of adaptive effects during adaptation to prism , 1965 .

[4]  R. Held,et al.  A Neural Model for Labile Sensorimotor Coordinations , 1962 .

[5]  S WAPNER,et al.  Experiments on sensory-tonic field theory of perception. VII. Effec of asymmetrical extent and starting positions of figures on the visual apparent median plane. , 1953, Journal of experimental psychology.

[6]  C. S. Harris Perceptual adaptation to inverted, reversed, and displaced vision. , 1965, Psychological review.

[7]  Samuel C. McLaughlin,et al.  Auditory localization and judgments of straight ahead during adaptation to prism , 1965 .

[8]  I. Kohler,et al.  The formation and transformation of the perceptual world. , 1963 .

[9]  C. S. Harris Adaptation to Displaced Vision: Visual, Motor, or Proprioceptive Change? , 1963, Science.

[10]  I P HOWARD,et al.  VISUOMOTOR ADAPTATION TO DISCORDANT EXAFFERENT STIMULATION. , 1965, Journal of experimental psychology.

[11]  E. Sersen,et al.  Is Reafference Necessary for Visual Adaptation? , 1964, Perceptual and motor skills.

[12]  R. Held,et al.  Neonatal deprivation and adult rearrangement: complementary techniques for analyzing plastic sensory-motor coordinations. , 1961, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[13]  H. Wallach,et al.  A PASSIVE CONDITION FOR RAPID ADAPTATION TO DISPLACED VISUAL DIRECTION. , 1963, The American journal of psychology.

[14]  J. Gibson The perception of the visual world , 1951 .

[15]  W. Bevan,et al.  THE CONCEPT OF ADAPTATION IN MODERN PSYCHOLOGY. , 1965, The Journal of psychology.

[16]  R H Day,et al.  Spatial adaptation and aftereffect with optically transformed vision: effects of active and passive responding and the relationship between test and exposure responses. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[17]  Richard Held,et al.  Technique for Studying Adaptation to Disarranged Hand-Eye Coordination , 1958 .

[18]  H. Pick,et al.  Visual and proprioceptive adaptation to optical displacement of the visual stimulus. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[19]  Margaret Wooster Certain factors in the development of a new spatial co-ordination. , 1923 .