Orientation perception in Williams Syndrome: Discrimination and integration
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Heather J. Chisum,et al. The contribution of vertical and horizontal connections to the receptive field center and surround in V1 , 2004, Neural Networks.
[2] Ursula Bellugi,et al. Reduced parietal and visual cortical activation during global processing in Williams syndrome , 2007, Developmental medicine and child neurology.
[3] W. D. Ross,et al. Visual brain and visual perception: how does the cortex do perceptual grouping? , 1997, Trends in Neurosciences.
[4] E. R. Siqueland,et al. Discrimination of Orientation by Human Infants , 1981, Perception.
[5] Paul M. Thompson,et al. 3D pattern of brain abnormalities in Williams syndrome visualized using tensor-based morphometry , 2007, NeuroImage.
[6] I. Kovács. Human development of perceptual organization , 2000, Vision Research.
[7] Barbara Landau,et al. Vision for perception and vision for action: normal and unusual development. , 2008, Developmental science.
[8] R. Malatesha,et al. Discrimination of mirror image stimuli in children. , 1974, Neuropsychologia.
[9] Daphne Maurer,et al. Orientation discrimination in 5-year-olds and adults tested with luminance-modulated and contrast-modulated gratings. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[10] O. Braddick,et al. A specific deficit of dorsal stream function in Williams' syndrome , 1997, Neuroreport.
[11] C. Gilbert,et al. Spatial integration and cortical dynamics. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[12] F. Campbell,et al. Neurophysiological Localization of the Vertical and Horizontal Visual Coordinates in Man , 1970, Science.
[13] R. Mansfield,et al. Neural Basis of Orientation Perception in Primate Vision , 1974, Science.
[14] Arnaud Cachia,et al. Parieto-occipital grey matter abnormalities in children with Williams syndrome , 2006, NeuroImage.
[15] M. McCloskey,et al. Representing orientation: A coordinate-system hypothesis and evidence from developmental deficits , 2006, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[16] G A Orban,et al. Preferences for horizontal or vertical orientation in cat visual cortical neurones [proceedings]. , 1979, The Journal of physiology.
[17] P. Strømme,et al. Prevalence Estimation of Williams Syndrome , 2002, Journal of child neurology.
[18] B N Timney,et al. Orientation anisotropy: incidence and magnitude in Caucasian and Chinese subjects. , 1976, Science.
[19] M. Corballis,et al. Winding one's ps and qs: mental rotation and mirror-image discrimination. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[20] Ursula Bellugi,et al. Bridging cognition, the brain and molecular genetics: evidence from Williams syndrome , 1999, Trends in Neurosciences.
[21] M. Carandini,et al. Neuronal Selectivity and Local Map Structure in Visual Cortex , 2008, Neuron.
[22] C. Furmanski,et al. An oblique effect in human primary visual cortex , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[23] Barbara Landau,et al. Motion processing specialization in Williams syndrome , 2005, Vision Research.
[24] A. Slater,et al. Orientation Discrimination and Cortical Function in the Human Newborn , 1988, Perception.
[25] M. Konishi,et al. Sexual differentiation of brain and behaviour , 1982, Trends in Neurosciences.
[26] Paul Schrater,et al. Shape perception reduces activity in human primary visual cortex , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[27] Janette Atkinson,et al. Neurobiological Models of Visuospatial Cognition in Children With Williams Syndrome: Measures of Dorsal-Stream and Frontal Function , 2003, Developmental neuropsychology.
[28] Gaia Scerif,et al. The dawn of cognitive genetics? Crucial developmental caveats , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[29] C. Mervis,et al. Neural Basis of Genetically Determined Visuospatial Construction Deficit in Williams Syndrome , 2004, Neuron.
[30] S. Thorpe,et al. Dynamics of orientation coding in area V1 of the awake primate , 1993, Visual Neuroscience.
[31] U Bellugi,et al. Williams syndrome and the brain. , 1997, Scientific American.
[32] L. Hildman,et al. Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test , 1993 .
[33] Kiralee M. Hayashi,et al. Abnormal Cortical Complexity and Thickness Profiles Mapped in Williams Syndrome , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[34] Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al. ‘What’ and ‘where’ in the human brain , 1994, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[35] Péter Szigetvári,et al. What and When? , 2019, Inauguration and Liturgical Kingship in the Long Twelfth Century.
[36] Yan Song,et al. An event-related potential study on perceptual learning in grating orientation discrimination , 2007, Neuroreport.
[37] C. Mervis,et al. Global Spatial Organization by Individuals with Williams Syndrome , 1999 .
[38] C. Mervis,et al. A genetic model for understanding higher order visual processing: functional interactions of the ventral visual stream in Williams syndrome. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.
[39] R. Held,et al. Anisotropic resolution in children's vision , 1984, Vision Research.
[40] Carolyn B. Mervis,et al. The Williams Syndrome Cognitive Profile , 2000, Brain and Cognition.
[41] Akos Feher,et al. Contour integration deficit in Williams Syndrome children , 2010 .
[42] Thomas F. Nugent,et al. Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[43] U. Polat,et al. Lateral interactions between spatial channels: Suppression and facilitation revealed by lateral masking experiments , 1993, Vision Research.
[44] F. C. Volkmann,et al. Visual discrimination of orientation by the goldfish (Carassius auratus). , 1974, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[45] E. Farran. Orientation Coding: A Specific Deficit in Williams Syndrome? , 2006, Developmental neuropsychology.
[46] B. Landau,et al. Visuospatial interpolation in typically developing children and in people with Williams Syndrome , 2008, Vision Research.
[47] C. Deruelle,et al. Configural and Local Processing of Faces in Children with Williams Syndrome , 1999, Brain and Cognition.
[48] E. Farran. Perceptual grouping ability in Williams syndrome: evidence for deviant patterns of performance , 2005, Neuropsychologia.
[49] Susan J. Lederman,et al. Multisensory Activation of the Intraparietal Area When Classifying Grating Orientation: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[50] M. Goodale,et al. Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.
[51] Ursula Bellugi,et al. Seeing either the forest or the trees: Dissociation in visuospatial processing , 1989, Brain and Cognition.
[52] D H Brainard,et al. The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[53] S. Appelle. Perception and discrimination as a function of stimulus orientation: the "oblique effect" in man and animals. , 1972, Psychological bulletin.
[54] David J. Field,et al. Contour integration by the human visual system: Evidence for a local “association field” , 1993, Vision Research.
[55] Ronald V. Schmelzer,et al. Differential Abilities Scales , 2008 .
[56] K. Wilmut,et al. Texture segmentation in Williams syndrome , 2007, Neuropsychologia.
[57] B. Landau,et al. Normal Susceptibility to Visual Illusions in Abnormal Development: Evidence from Williams Syndrome , 2009, Perception.
[58] H. Bülthoff,et al. Perceptual Organization of Local Elements into Global Shapes in the Human Visual Cortex , 2003, Current Biology.
[59] J. Hoffman,et al. Spatial breakdown in spatial construction: Evidence from eye fixations in children with Williams syndrome , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.
[60] J. Atkinson,et al. Normal and anomalous development of visual motion processing: motion coherence and ‘dorsal-stream vulnerability’ , 2003, Neuropsychologia.
[61] D G Pelli,et al. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.