Orientation perception in Williams Syndrome: Discrimination and integration

Williams Syndrome (WS) is a rare neurodevelopmental disorder, which stems from a genetic deletion on chromosome 7 and causes a profound weakness in visuospatial cognition. Our current study explores how orientation perception may contribute to the visuospatial deficits in WS. In Experiment 1, we found that WS individuals and normal 3-4 year olds had similar orientation discrimination thresholds and had similar prevalence of mirror-reversal errors for diagonal targets (+/-45 deg). In Experiment 2, we asked whether this immaturity in orientation discrimination would also be reflected in a task requiring integration of oriented elements. We found that sensitivities of WS individuals for detecting orientation-defined contours were higher than sensitivities of normal 3-4 year olds, and were not significantly different from sensitivities of normal adults. Together, these results suggest that orientation discrimination and orientation integration have different maturational trajectories in normal development and different susceptibilities to damage in WS. These may reflect largely separate visuospatial mechanisms.

[1]  Heather J. Chisum,et al.  The contribution of vertical and horizontal connections to the receptive field center and surround in V1 , 2004, Neural Networks.

[2]  Ursula Bellugi,et al.  Reduced parietal and visual cortical activation during global processing in Williams syndrome , 2007, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[3]  W. D. Ross,et al.  Visual brain and visual perception: how does the cortex do perceptual grouping? , 1997, Trends in Neurosciences.

[4]  E. R. Siqueland,et al.  Discrimination of Orientation by Human Infants , 1981, Perception.

[5]  Paul M. Thompson,et al.  3D pattern of brain abnormalities in Williams syndrome visualized using tensor-based morphometry , 2007, NeuroImage.

[6]  I. Kovács Human development of perceptual organization , 2000, Vision Research.

[7]  Barbara Landau,et al.  Vision for perception and vision for action: normal and unusual development. , 2008, Developmental science.

[8]  R. Malatesha,et al.  Discrimination of mirror image stimuli in children. , 1974, Neuropsychologia.

[9]  Daphne Maurer,et al.  Orientation discrimination in 5-year-olds and adults tested with luminance-modulated and contrast-modulated gratings. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[10]  O. Braddick,et al.  A specific deficit of dorsal stream function in Williams' syndrome , 1997, Neuroreport.

[11]  C. Gilbert,et al.  Spatial integration and cortical dynamics. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[12]  F. Campbell,et al.  Neurophysiological Localization of the Vertical and Horizontal Visual Coordinates in Man , 1970, Science.

[13]  R. Mansfield,et al.  Neural Basis of Orientation Perception in Primate Vision , 1974, Science.

[14]  Arnaud Cachia,et al.  Parieto-occipital grey matter abnormalities in children with Williams syndrome , 2006, NeuroImage.

[15]  M. McCloskey,et al.  Representing orientation: A coordinate-system hypothesis and evidence from developmental deficits , 2006, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[16]  G A Orban,et al.  Preferences for horizontal or vertical orientation in cat visual cortical neurones [proceedings]. , 1979, The Journal of physiology.

[17]  P. Strømme,et al.  Prevalence Estimation of Williams Syndrome , 2002, Journal of child neurology.

[18]  B N Timney,et al.  Orientation anisotropy: incidence and magnitude in Caucasian and Chinese subjects. , 1976, Science.

[19]  M. Corballis,et al.  Winding one's ps and qs: mental rotation and mirror-image discrimination. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  Ursula Bellugi,et al.  Bridging cognition, the brain and molecular genetics: evidence from Williams syndrome , 1999, Trends in Neurosciences.

[21]  M. Carandini,et al.  Neuronal Selectivity and Local Map Structure in Visual Cortex , 2008, Neuron.

[22]  C. Furmanski,et al.  An oblique effect in human primary visual cortex , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  Barbara Landau,et al.  Motion processing specialization in Williams syndrome , 2005, Vision Research.

[24]  A. Slater,et al.  Orientation Discrimination and Cortical Function in the Human Newborn , 1988, Perception.

[25]  M. Konishi,et al.  Sexual differentiation of brain and behaviour , 1982, Trends in Neurosciences.

[26]  Paul Schrater,et al.  Shape perception reduces activity in human primary visual cortex , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[27]  Janette Atkinson,et al.  Neurobiological Models of Visuospatial Cognition in Children With Williams Syndrome: Measures of Dorsal-Stream and Frontal Function , 2003, Developmental neuropsychology.

[28]  Gaia Scerif,et al.  The dawn of cognitive genetics? Crucial developmental caveats , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[29]  C. Mervis,et al.  Neural Basis of Genetically Determined Visuospatial Construction Deficit in Williams Syndrome , 2004, Neuron.

[30]  S. Thorpe,et al.  Dynamics of orientation coding in area V1 of the awake primate , 1993, Visual Neuroscience.

[31]  U Bellugi,et al.  Williams syndrome and the brain. , 1997, Scientific American.

[32]  L. Hildman,et al.  Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test , 1993 .

[33]  Kiralee M. Hayashi,et al.  Abnormal Cortical Complexity and Thickness Profiles Mapped in Williams Syndrome , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  ‘What’ and ‘where’ in the human brain , 1994, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[35]  Péter Szigetvári,et al.  What and When? , 2019, Inauguration and Liturgical Kingship in the Long Twelfth Century.

[36]  Yan Song,et al.  An event-related potential study on perceptual learning in grating orientation discrimination , 2007, Neuroreport.

[37]  C. Mervis,et al.  Global Spatial Organization by Individuals with Williams Syndrome , 1999 .

[38]  C. Mervis,et al.  A genetic model for understanding higher order visual processing: functional interactions of the ventral visual stream in Williams syndrome. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[39]  R. Held,et al.  Anisotropic resolution in children's vision , 1984, Vision Research.

[40]  Carolyn B. Mervis,et al.  The Williams Syndrome Cognitive Profile , 2000, Brain and Cognition.

[41]  Akos Feher,et al.  Contour integration deficit in Williams Syndrome children , 2010 .

[42]  Thomas F. Nugent,et al.  Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[43]  U. Polat,et al.  Lateral interactions between spatial channels: Suppression and facilitation revealed by lateral masking experiments , 1993, Vision Research.

[44]  F. C. Volkmann,et al.  Visual discrimination of orientation by the goldfish (Carassius auratus). , 1974, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[45]  E. Farran Orientation Coding: A Specific Deficit in Williams Syndrome? , 2006, Developmental neuropsychology.

[46]  B. Landau,et al.  Visuospatial interpolation in typically developing children and in people with Williams Syndrome , 2008, Vision Research.

[47]  C. Deruelle,et al.  Configural and Local Processing of Faces in Children with Williams Syndrome , 1999, Brain and Cognition.

[48]  E. Farran Perceptual grouping ability in Williams syndrome: evidence for deviant patterns of performance , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[49]  Susan J. Lederman,et al.  Multisensory Activation of the Intraparietal Area When Classifying Grating Orientation: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[50]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[51]  Ursula Bellugi,et al.  Seeing either the forest or the trees: Dissociation in visuospatial processing , 1989, Brain and Cognition.

[52]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[53]  S. Appelle Perception and discrimination as a function of stimulus orientation: the "oblique effect" in man and animals. , 1972, Psychological bulletin.

[54]  David J. Field,et al.  Contour integration by the human visual system: Evidence for a local “association field” , 1993, Vision Research.

[55]  Ronald V. Schmelzer,et al.  Differential Abilities Scales , 2008 .

[56]  K. Wilmut,et al.  Texture segmentation in Williams syndrome , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[57]  B. Landau,et al.  Normal Susceptibility to Visual Illusions in Abnormal Development: Evidence from Williams Syndrome , 2009, Perception.

[58]  H. Bülthoff,et al.  Perceptual Organization of Local Elements into Global Shapes in the Human Visual Cortex , 2003, Current Biology.

[59]  J. Hoffman,et al.  Spatial breakdown in spatial construction: Evidence from eye fixations in children with Williams syndrome , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.

[60]  J. Atkinson,et al.  Normal and anomalous development of visual motion processing: motion coherence and ‘dorsal-stream vulnerability’ , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[61]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.