Early top-down influences on bistable perception revealed by event-related potentials
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] S. Hillyard,et al. Selective attention to the color and direction of moving stimuli: Electrophysiological correlates of hierarchical feature selection , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.
[2] G. J. Brouwer,et al. Voluntary control and the dynamics of perceptual bi-stability , 2005, Vision Research.
[3] David L. Sheinberg,et al. The role of temporal cortical areas in perceptual organization. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[4] Sabine Windmann,et al. Role of the Prefrontal Cortex in Attentional Control over Bistable Vision , 2006 .
[5] S. Luck,et al. Spatio‐temporal dynamics of attention to color: Evidence from human electrophysiology , 1998, Human brain mapping.
[6] L. Cohen,et al. Rate of Apparent Change of a Necker Cube as a Function of Prior Stimulation , 1959 .
[7] Bernice Porjesz,et al. P300: The Similarities and Differences in the Scalp Distribution of Visual and Auditory Modality , 2004, Brain Topography.
[8] N. Kawabata. Attention and Depth Perception , 1986, Perception.
[9] Thomas F. Nugent,et al. Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[10] S. Yantis,et al. Common neural substrates for the control and effects of visual attention and perceptual bistability. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[11] Thomas C. Toppino,et al. Selective adaptation with reversible figures: Don’t change that channel , 1987, Perception & psychophysics.
[12] Ahmet Ademoglu,et al. Analysis of the electroencephalographic activity during the Necker cube reversals by means of the wavelet transform , 1998, Biological Cybernetics.
[13] Michael Bach,et al. Early neural activity in Necker-cube reversal: evidence for low-level processing of a gestalt phenomenon. , 2004, Psychophysiology.
[14] Michael A. Pitts,et al. Electrophysiological correlates of perceptual reversals for three different types of multistable images. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[15] R. Liebert,et al. Voluntary Control of Reversible Figures , 1985, Perceptual and motor skills.
[16] H. Heinze,et al. An event-related brain potential study of visual selective attention to conjunctions of color and shape. , 1999, Psychophysiology.
[17] T W Picton,et al. The P300 Wave of the Human Event‐Related Potential , 1992, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.
[18] Laurie A. Miller,et al. Perceptual flexibility after frontal or temporal lobectomy , 1994, Neuropsychologia.
[19] Thomas C. Toppino,et al. Prime time: Fatigue and set effects in the perception of reversible figures , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.
[20] A. Wolters,et al. Dynamics in Psychology , 1943, Nature.
[21] S. Hillyard,et al. Event-related brain potentials in the study of visual selective attention. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[22] M. Valdés-Sosa,et al. Switching Attention without Shifting the Spotlight: Object-Based Attentional Modulation of Brain Potentials , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[23] S. Hillyard,et al. Spatial Selective Attention Affects Early Extrastriate But Not Striate Components of the Visual Evoked Potential , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[24] T. Jernigan,et al. Maturation of human cerebrum observed in vivo during adolescence. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[25] J. Fuster. Frontal lobe and cognitive development , 2002, Journal of neurocytology.
[26] J. Orbach,et al. Reversibility of the Necker Cube: I. An Examination of the Concept of “Satiation of Orientation” , 1963, Perceptual and motor skills.
[27] A. Treisman,et al. A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.
[28] V. Lamme,et al. The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.
[29] Steven A. Hillyard,et al. Effects of spatial cuing on luminance detectability: Psychophysical and electrophysiological evidence for early selection. , 1994 .
[30] John Harris,et al. Biasing Effects in Ambiguous Figures: Removal or Fixation of Critical Features Can Affect Perception , 1997 .
[31] N. Squires,et al. Visual evoked potentials to illusory reversals of the necker cube. , 1988, Psychophysiology.
[32] G. Mangun. Neural mechanisms of visual selective attention. , 1995, Psychophysiology.
[33] E. Vogel,et al. Sensory gain control (amplification) as a mechanism of selective attention: electrophysiological and neuroimaging evidence. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[34] G. Long,et al. How to Keep a Reversible Figure from Reversing: Teasing Out Top — Down and Bottom — Up Processes , 2007, Perception.
[35] Michael Bach,et al. The Necker cube—an ambiguous figure disambiguated in early visual processing , 2005, Vision Research.
[36] Raymond van Ee,et al. Dynamics of perceptual bi-stability for stereoscopic slant rivalry and a comparison with grating, house-face, and Necker cube rivalry , 2005, Vision Research.
[37] A. Toga,et al. In vivo evidence for post-adolescent brain maturation in frontal and striatal regions , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.
[38] Krista L. Horlitz,et al. Satiation or availability? Effects of attention, memory, and imagery on the perception of ambiguous figures , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.
[39] A. Toga,et al. Mapping Continued Brain Growth and Gray Matter Density Reduction in Dorsal Frontal Cortex: Inverse Relationships during Postadolescent Brain Maturation , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[40] N. Logothetis,et al. Neuronal correlates of subjective visual perception. , 1989, Science.
[41] Thomas C. Toppino,et al. Enduring interest in perceptual ambiguity: alternating views of reversible figures. , 2004, Psychological bulletin.
[42] C. Blundo,et al. Perception of ambiguous figures after focal brain lesions , 1990, Neuropsychologia.
[43] G. Shulman. Attentional effects on Necker cube adaptation , 1993 .
[44] N. Logothetis,et al. Activity changes in early visual cortex reflect monkeys' percepts during binocular rivalry , 1996, Nature.
[45] L. H. Pelton,et al. Acceleration of reversals of a Necker cube. , 1968, The American journal of psychology.
[46] Insights from event-related potentials into the temporal and hierarchical organization of the ventral and dorsal streams of the visual system in selective attention. , 1999, Psychophysiology.
[47] I Rock,et al. Why do ambiguous figures reverse? , 1994, Acta psychologica.
[48] Alan C. Evans,et al. Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.
[49] N. Logothetis,et al. Multistable phenomena: changing views in perception , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[50] D. Strüber,et al. Differences in Top—Down Influences on the Reversal Rate of Different Categories of Reversible Figures , 1999, Perception.
[51] E Başar,et al. Multistable visual perception induces a slow positive EEG wave. , 1993, The International journal of neuroscience.
[52] E. Donchin,et al. Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[53] H G Smid,et al. The organization of multidimensional selection on the basis of color and shape: An event-related brain potential study , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.
[54] F. Tong,et al. Can attention selectively bias bistable perception? Differences between binocular rivalry and ambiguous figures. , 2004, Journal of vision.
[55] Joel L. Davis,et al. Visual attention and cortical circuits , 2001 .
[56] Thomas C. Toppino,et al. Reversible-figure perception: Mechanisms of intentional control , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.
[57] F. Karayanidis,et al. An exploration of varieties of visual attention: ERP findings. , 1999, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.