Improving Preschoolers’ Arithmetic through Number Magnitude Training: The Impact of Non-Symbolic and Symbolic Training

The numerical cognition literature offers two views to explain numerical and arithmetical development. The unique-representation view considers the approximate number system (ANS) to represent the magnitude of both symbolic and non-symbolic numbers and to be the basis of numerical learning. In contrast, the dual-representation view suggests that symbolic and non-symbolic skills rely on different magnitude representations and that it is the ability to build an exact representation of symbolic numbers that underlies math learning. Support for these hypotheses has come mainly from correlative studies with inconsistent results. In this study, we developed two training programs aiming at enhancing the magnitude processing of either non-symbolic numbers or symbolic numbers and compared their effects on arithmetic skills. Fifty-six preschoolers were randomly assigned to one of three 10-session-training conditions: (1) non-symbolic training (2) symbolic training and (3) control training working on story understanding. Both numerical training conditions were significantly more efficient than the control condition in improving magnitude processing. Moreover, symbolic training led to a significantly larger improvement in arithmetic than did non-symbolic training and the control condition. These results support the dual-representation view.

[1]  Emmy Defever,et al.  The Approximate Number System is not Predictive for Symbolic Number Processing in Kindergarteners , 2014, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[2]  Marie-Pascale Noël,et al.  Developmental Changes in the Profiles of Dyscalculia: An Explanation Based on a Double Exact-and-Approximate Number Representation Model , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[3]  Wim Fias,et al.  Priming reveals differential coding of symbolic and non-symbolic quantities , 2007, Cognition.

[4]  B. Reynvoet,et al.  Enhancing arithmetic in pre-schoolers with comparison or number line estimation training: Does it matter? ☆ , 2016 .

[5]  Hilary Barth,et al.  Abstract number and arithmetic in preschool children. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[6]  J. Bulthé,et al.  Format-dependent representations of symbolic and non-symbolic numbers in the human cortex as revealed by multi-voxel pattern analyses , 2014, NeuroImage.

[7]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Number sense across the lifespan as revealed by a massive Internet-based sample , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[8]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  Symbolic arithmetic knowledge without instruction , 2007, Nature.

[9]  M. Zorzi,et al.  Training numerical skills with the adaptive videogame “The Number Race”: A randomized controlled trial on preschoolers , 2016, Trends in Neuroscience and Education.

[10]  Daniel C. Hyde,et al.  Brief non-symbolic, approximate number practice enhances subsequent exact symbolic arithmetic in children , 2014, Cognition.

[11]  E. Spelke,et al.  The construction of large number representations in adults , 2003, Cognition.

[12]  Bert De Smedt,et al.  Defective number module or impaired access? Numerical magnitude processing in first graders with mathematical difficulties. , 2011, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[13]  E. Spelke,et al.  Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants , 2000, Cognition.

[14]  Susan Carey,et al.  One, two, three, four, nothing more: An investigation of the conceptual sources of the verbal counting principles , 2007, Cognition.

[15]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Links Between the Intuitive Sense of Number and Formal Mathematics Ability. , 2013, Child development perspectives.

[16]  Michael Schneider,et al.  Associations of non-symbolic and symbolic numerical magnitude processing with mathematical competence: a meta-analysis. , 2017, Developmental science.

[17]  Fei Xu,et al.  Numerosity discrimination in infants: Evidence for two systems of representations , 2003, Cognition.

[18]  Bert Reynvoet,et al.  Evidence for distinct magnitude systems for symbolic and non-symbolic number , 2017, Psychological research.

[19]  Emmy Defever,et al.  Association between basic numerical abilities and mathematics achievement. , 2012, The British journal of developmental psychology.

[20]  Andy P. Field,et al.  Discovering Statistics Using SPSS , 2000 .

[21]  D. Ansari,et al.  Symbolic Numerical Magnitude Processing Is as Important to Arithmetic as Phonological Awareness Is to Reading , 2016, PloS one.

[22]  Korbinian Moeller,et al.  Walk the number line – An embodied training of numerical concepts , 2013, Trends in Neuroscience and Education.

[23]  Nicholas K DeWind,et al.  Using cognitive training studies to unravel the mechanisms by which the approximate number system supports symbolic math ability , 2016, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.

[24]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Effects of an Adaptive Game Intervention on Accessing Number Sense in Low-Socioeconomic-Status Kindergarten Children , 2009 .

[25]  P. Gordon Numerical Cognition Without Words: Evidence from Amazonia , 2004, Science.

[26]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Abstract representations of numbers in the animal and human brain , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.

[27]  Neil Marlow,et al.  Individual Differences in Inhibitory Control, Not Non-Verbal Number Acuity, Correlate with Mathematics Achievement , 2013, PloS one.

[28]  Bert Reynvoet,et al.  The interplay between nonsymbolic number and its continuous visual properties. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[29]  Stefan Ufer,et al.  How Training on Exact or Approximate Mental Representations of Number Can Enhance First-Grade Students' Basic Number Processing and Arithmetic Skills , 2013 .

[30]  Michael von Aster,et al.  Mental number line training in children with developmental dyscalculia , 2011, NeuroImage.

[31]  Susan Carey,et al.  Cognitive Foundations of Arithmetic: Evolution and Ontogenisis , 2001 .

[32]  Paul B. Buckley,et al.  Comparisons of digits and dot patterns. , 1974, Journal of experimental psychology.

[33]  Lisa K Fazio,et al.  Relations of different types of numerical magnitude representations to each other and to mathematics achievement. , 2014, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[34]  E. Spelke,et al.  Language and Conceptual Development series Core systems of number , 2004 .

[35]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Exact and Approximate Arithmetic in an Amazonian Indigene Group , 2004, Science.

[36]  Laurence Rousselle,et al.  Magnitude Representations in Williams Syndrome: Differential Acuity in Time, Space and Number Processing , 2013, PloS one.

[37]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Impaired acuity of the approximate number system underlies mathematical learning disability (dyscalculia). , 2011, Child development.

[38]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Developmental change in the acuity of the "Number Sense": The Approximate Number System in 3-, 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds and adults. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[39]  Marc D Hauser,et al.  Evolutionary foundations of number: spontaneous representation of numerical magnitudes by cotton–top tamarins , 2003, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[40]  Ian M. Lyons,et al.  Foundations of children's numerical and mathematical skills: the roles of symbolic and nonsymbolic representations of numerical magnitude. , 2015, Advances in child development and behavior.

[41]  A. Desoete,et al.  Teaching and Teacher Education , 2014 .

[42]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Individual differences in non-verbal number acuity correlate with maths achievement , 2008, Nature.

[43]  Elizabeth S Spelke,et al.  Origins of Number Sense , 2003, Psychological science.

[44]  Manuela Piazza,et al.  Neurocognitive start-up tools for symbolic number representations , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[45]  R. Cohen Kadosh,et al.  Sensory-integration system rather than approximate number system underlies numerosity processing: A critical review. , 2016, Acta psychologica.

[46]  Xavier Seron,et al.  Over-estimation in numerosity estimation tasks: more than an attentional bias? , 2012, Acta psychologica.

[47]  H. Bawden,et al.  Human Behavior, Learning, and the Developing Brain: Atypical Development , 2010 .

[48]  G. Burghardt,et al.  Playing Linear Number Board Games — But Not Circular Ones — Improves Low-Income Preschoolers ’ Numerical Understanding , 2009 .

[49]  C. Gilmore,et al.  Children's mapping between symbolic and nonsymbolic representations of number. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[50]  Bert De Smedt,et al.  Numerical Magnitude Representations and Individual Differences in Children's Arithmetic Strategy Use , 2012 .

[51]  S. Dehaene,et al.  An open trial assessment of "The Number Race", an adaptive computer game for remediation of dyscalculia , 2006, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[52]  Marie-Pascale Noël,et al.  Symbolic and nonsymbolic number comparison in children with and without dyscalculia , 2010, Cognition.

[53]  P. Cirino The interrelationships of mathematical precursors in kindergarten. , 2011, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[54]  Manuela Piazza,et al.  How Humans Count: Numerosity and the Parietal Cortex , 2009, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[55]  Stéphane Rusinek,et al.  L’outil « estimateur », la ligne numérique mentale et les habiletés arithmétiques , 2010 .

[56]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Computer-Assisted Intervention for Children with Low Numeracy Skills , 2009 .

[57]  S. Carey Bootstrapping & the origin of concepts , 2004, Daedalus.

[58]  Pierre Pica,et al.  Education Enhances the Acuity of the Nonverbal Approximate Number System , 2013, Psychological science.

[59]  Evelyn H. Kroesbergen,et al.  Early Numerical Development and the Role of Non-Symbolic and Symbolic Skills. , 2013 .

[60]  Andrea Facoetti,et al.  Developmental trajectory of number acuity reveals a severe impairment in developmental dyscalculia , 2010, Cognition.

[61]  S. Carey The Origin of Concepts , 2000 .

[62]  M. Noël,et al.  Basic numerical skills in children with mathematics learning disabilities: A comparison of symbolic vs non-symbolic number magnitude processing , 2007, Cognition.

[63]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  How do symbolic and non-symbolic numerical magnitude processing skills relate to individual differences in children's mathematical skills? A review of evidence from brain and behavior , 2013, Trends in Neuroscience and Education.

[64]  E. Spelke,et al.  Newborn infants perceive abstract numbers , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[65]  M. Posner,et al.  Brain mechanisms of quantity are similar in 5-year-old children and adults. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[66]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Mapping numerical magnitudes onto symbols: the numerical distance effect and individual differences in children's mathematics achievement. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[67]  D. Ansari,et al.  Trajectories of Symbolic and Nonsymbolic Magnitude Processing in the First Year of Formal Schooling , 2016, PloS one.

[68]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Rethinking the implications of numerical ratio effects for understanding the development of representational precision and numerical processing across formats. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[69]  Melissa E. Libertus,et al.  Behavioral and Neural Basis of Number Sense in Infancy , 2009, Current directions in psychological science.

[70]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Principles underlying the design of "The Number Race", an adaptive computer game for remediation of dyscalculia , 2006, Behavioral and brain functions : BBF.

[71]  Jessica Sullivan,et al.  Children's mappings of large number words to numerosities , 2009 .

[72]  S. Dehaene,et al.  A Magnitude Code Common to Numerosities and Number Symbols in Human Intraparietal Cortex , 2007, Neuron.

[73]  Michael W. L. Chee,et al.  Neural correlates of symbolic and non-symbolic arithmetic , 2005, Neuropsychologia.