The Role of Implicit Context Information in Guiding Visual-Spatial Attention
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Paolo Traverso,et al. Automated Planning: Theory & Practice , 2004 .
[2] Jeffrey S. Johnson,et al. Implicit memory influences the allocation of attention in visual cortex , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[3] Thomas Dean,et al. Automated planning , 1996, CSUR.
[4] M. Chun,et al. Memory deficits for implicit contextual information in amnesic subjects with hippocampal damage , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.
[5] M. Shaw,et al. Optimal allocation of cognitive resources to spatial locations. , 1977, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[6] W. Prinz. Perception and Action Planning , 1997 .
[7] A. Schubö,et al. Cognitive processes facilitated by contextual cueing: evidence from event-related brain potentials. , 2009, Psychophysiology.
[8] P. Perona,et al. Why does natural scene categorization require little attention? Exploring attentional requirements for natural and synthetic stimuli , 2005 .
[9] K. Nakayama,et al. Priming of popout: II. Role of position , 1996 .
[10] Kevin J. Hawley,et al. Attention capture by novel stimuli. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[11] Michael A. Stadler,et al. Handbook of implicit learning , 1998 .
[12] T Allison,et al. Contextual guidance of attention: human intracranial event-related potential evidence for feedback modulation in anatomically early temporally late stages of visual processing. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[13] M. Nissen,et al. Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.
[14] A. Reber. Implicit learning and tacit knowledge , 1993 .
[15] M. Bravo,et al. The role of attention in different visual-search tasks , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.
[16] Scott T. Grafton,et al. Premotor Cortex Activation during Observation and Naming of Familiar Tools , 1997, NeuroImage.
[17] M. Chun,et al. Top-Down Attentional Guidance Based on Implicit Learning of Visual Covariation , 1999 .
[18] James R. Brockmole,et al. Using real-world scenes as contextual cues for search , 2006 .
[19] S. Yantis,et al. Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Evidence from visual search. , 1984 .
[20] William L. Gross,et al. Hippocampal differentiation without recognition: an fMRI analysis of the contextual cueing task. , 2007, Learning & memory.
[21] A. Greenwald,et al. A choice reaction time test of ideomotor theory. , 1970, Journal of experimental psychology.
[22] Denis Fize,et al. Speed of processing in the human visual system , 1996, Nature.
[23] Bernhard Hommel,et al. Codes and their vicissitudes , 2001 .
[24] M. Chun,et al. Contextual Cueing: Implicit Learning and Memory of Visual Context Guides Spatial Attention , 1998, Cognitive Psychology.
[25] P Cavanagh,et al. Familiarity and pop-out in visual search , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.
[26] M. Chun,et al. Implicit, long-term spatial contextual memory. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[27] K. Nakayama,et al. Priming of pop-out: II. The role of position , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.
[28] Marilyn L. Shaw,et al. A capacity allocation model for reaction time. , 1978 .
[29] Melina A. Kunar,et al. Contextual cuing by global features , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.
[30] E Tulving,et al. Priming and human memory systems. , 1990, Science.
[31] James R. Brockmole,et al. Contextual cueing in naturalistic scenes: Global and local contexts. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[32] G. Aschersleben,et al. The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.
[33] K. Nakayama,et al. Priming of pop-out: I. Role of features , 1994, Memory & cognition.
[34] A Treisman,et al. Feature analysis in early vision: evidence from search asymmetries. , 1988, Psychological review.