Perseveration and Strategy in a Novel Spatial Self-Ordered Sequencing Task for Nonhuman Primates: Effects of Excitotoxic Lesions and Dopamine Depletions of the Prefrontal Cortex

Damage to the prefrontal cortex disrupts the performance of self-ordered sequencing tasks, although the precise mechanisms by which this effect occurs is unclear. Active working memory, inhibitory control, and the ability to generate and perform a sequence of responses are all putative cognitive abilities that may be responsible for the impaired performance that results from disruption of prefrontal processing. In addition, the neurochemical substrates underlying prefrontal cognitive function are not well understood, although active working memory appears to depend upon an intact mesocortical dopamine system. The present experiments were therefore designed to evaluate explicitly the contribution of each of these abilities to successful performance of a novel spatial self-ordered sequencing task and to examine the contribution of the prefrontal cortex and its dopamine innervation to each ability in turn. Excitotoxic lesions of the prefrontal cortex of the common marmoset profoundly impaired the performance of the self-ordered sequencing task and induced robust perseverative responding. Task manipulations that precluded perseveration ameliorated the effect of this lesion and revealed that the ability to generate and perform sequences of responses was unaffected by excitotoxic damage to prefrontal cortex. In contrast, large dopamine and noradrenaline depletions within the same areas of prefrontal cortex had no effect on any aspect of the self-ordered task but did impair the acquisition of an active working memory task, spatial delayed response, to the same degree as the excitotoxic lesion. These results demonstrate that a lesion of the ascending monoamine projections to the pre-frontal cortex is not always synonymous with a lesion of the prefrontal cortex itself and thereby challenge existing concepts concerning the neuromodulation of prefrontal cognitive function.

[1]  Robert Freedman,et al.  Effects of putative neurotransmitters on neuronal activity in monkey auditory cortex , 1975, Brain Research.

[2]  K. Pribram,et al.  Analysis of the effects of frontal lesions in monkey. II. Variations of delayed response. , 1956, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[3]  Marc D Hauser,et al.  Perseveration, inhibition and the prefrontal cortex: a new look , 1999, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[4]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  The alpha-2 adrenergic agonist guanfacine improves memory in aged monkeys without sedative or hypotensive side effects: evidence for alpha-2 receptor subtypes , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[5]  T. A. Jackson,et al.  AN EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF THE FUNCTIONS OF THE FRONTAL ASSOCIATION AREAS IN PRIMATES * 1 2 , 1935 .

[6]  J. M. Warren,et al.  THE FRONTAL GRANULAR CORTEX AND BEHAVIOR , 1964 .

[7]  J. Fulton,et al.  Frontal lobe ablation in chimpanzee; a resume of Becky and Lucy. , 1948, Research publications - Association for Research in Nervous and Mental Disease.

[8]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Alpha 2-adrenergic mechanisms in prefrontal cortex associated with cognitive decline in aged nonhuman primates. , 1985, Science.

[9]  M Freedman,et al.  Bilateral frontal lobe disease and selective delayed response deficits in humans. , 1986, Behavioral neuroscience.

[10]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Evidence for a two-stage model of spatial working memory processing within the lateral frontal cortex: a positron emission tomography study. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[11]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Limbic lesions and the problem of stimulus--reinforcement associations. , 1972, Experimental neurology.

[12]  R E Passingham Memory of monkeys (Macaca mulatta) with lesions in prefrontal cortex. , 1985, Behavioral neuroscience.

[13]  B. Sahakian,et al.  Does visuospatial memory in senile dementia of the Alzheimer type depend on the severity of the disorder? , 1992 .

[14]  E. Rolls,et al.  Emotion-related learning in patients with social and emotional changes associated with frontal lobe damage. , 1994, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[15]  C. Marsden,et al.  Comparison of executive and visuospatial memory function in Huntington's disease and dementia of Alzheimer type matched for degree of dementia. , 1995, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[16]  T. Robbins,et al.  Planning and spatial working memory following frontal lobe lesions in man , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Functional Anatomy of Visuomotor Skill Learning in Human Subjects Examined with Positron Emission Tomography , 1996, The European journal of neuroscience.

[18]  T. Robbins,et al.  Dissociation in prefrontal cortex of affective and attentional shifts , 1996, Nature.

[19]  M Petrides,et al.  Impairments on nonspatial self-ordered and externally ordered working memory tasks after lesions of the mid-dorsal part of the lateral frontal cortex in the monkey , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[20]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Non-spatial memory after selective prefrontal lesions in monkeys , 1978, Brain Research.

[21]  C. Jacobsen,et al.  Studies of cerebral function in primates. I. The functions of the frontal association areas in monkeys. , 1936 .

[22]  R. A. Blum Effects of subtotal lesions of frontal granular cortex on delayed reaction in monkeys. , 1952, A.M.A. archives of neurology and psychiatry.

[23]  Charles M. Butter,et al.  Perseveration in extinction and in discrimination reversal tasks following selective frontal ablations in Macaca mulatta , 1969 .

[24]  T. Robbins,et al.  The effects of intradimensional and extradimensional shifts on visual discrimination learning in humans and non-human primates , 1988, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[25]  B. Milner,et al.  Deficits on subject-ordered tasks after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in man , 1982, Neuropsychologia.

[26]  J. Hyttel,et al.  Citalopram — Pharmacological profile of a specific serotonin uptake inhibitor with antidepressant activity , 1982, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry.

[27]  T. Shallice Specific impairments of planning. , 1982, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[28]  M Petrides,et al.  Monitoring of selections of visual stimuli and the primate frontal cortex , 1991, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[29]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Conditioning and extinction of a food-rewarded response after selective ablations of frontal cortex in rhesus monkeys. , 1963, Experimental neurology.

[30]  K. Pribram,et al.  Effects on delayed-response performance of lesions of dorsolateral and ventromedial frontal cortex of baboons. , 1952, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[31]  Adrian M. Owen,et al.  Visuo-spatial short-term recognition memory and learning after temporal lobe excisions, frontal lobe excisions or amygdalo-hippocampectomy in man , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[32]  K. Pribram,et al.  Analysis of the effects of frontal lesions in monkey. III. Object alternation. , 1956, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[33]  A. C. Cuello,et al.  The brain of the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) a stereotaxic atlas H. Stephan,G. Baron &W. K. Schwerdtfeger. 1980. Springer-Verlag, Berlin. 5 figs., 3 tab., 73 plates. V 91 pages. Cloth DM 168,-; approx. US $94.10 , 1981, Neuroscience.

[34]  A C Roberts,et al.  6-Hydroxydopamine lesions of the prefrontal cortex in monkeys enhance performance on an analog of the Wisconsin Card Sort Test: possible interactions with subcortical dopamine , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[35]  B. Bunney,et al.  Pharmacological characterization of the receptor mediating electrophysiological responses to dopamine in the rat medial prefrontal cortex: a microiontophoretic study. , 1989, The Journal of pharmacology and experimental therapeutics.

[36]  Donald A. Norman,et al.  Attention to Action , 1986 .

[37]  T. Robbins,et al.  A specific form of cognitive rigidity following excitotoxic lesions of the basal forebrain in marmosets , 1992, Neuroscience.

[38]  Mortimer Mishkin,et al.  A re-examination of the effects of frontal lesions on object alternation , 1969 .

[39]  J. Glowinski,et al.  Behavioural deficits induced by an electrolytic lesion of the rat ventral mesencephalic tegmentum are corrected by a superimposed lesion of the dorsal noradrenergic system , 1988, Brain Research.

[40]  M MISHKIN,et al.  Effects of small frontal lesions on delayed alternation in monkeys. , 1957, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  A C Roberts,et al.  Primate analogue of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: effects of excitotoxic lesions of the prefrontal cortex in the marmoset. , 1996, Behavioral neuroscience.

[42]  N Butters,et al.  Retention of Delayed-Alternation: Effect of Selective Lesions of Sulcus Principalis , 1969, Science.

[43]  D. Norman,et al.  Attention to action: Willed and automatic control , 1980 .

[44]  P. Goldman-Rakic Dopamine-mediated mechanisms of the prefrontal cortex , 1992 .

[45]  Analysis of simple searching behavior in the frontal monkey. , 1958, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[46]  A C Roberts,et al.  Performance norms for a rhesus monkey neuropsychological testing battery: acquisition and long-term performance. , 1999, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[47]  M. Petrides Functional specialization within the dorsolateral frontal cortex for serial order memory , 1991, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[48]  M. Petrides Functional specialization within the primate dorsolateral frontal cortex. , 1992, Advances in neurology.

[49]  A. Arnsten Catecholamine modulation of prefrontal cortical cognitive function , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[50]  Tim Shallice,et al.  Supervisory control of action and thought selection. , 1993 .

[51]  Y. Agid,et al.  Delayed response tasks and prefrontal lesions in man—Evidence for self generated patterns of behaviour with poor environmental modulation , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[52]  T. Robbins,et al.  Frontal-striatal cognitive deficits in patients with chronic schizophrenia. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[53]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Dissociation of human mid-dorsolateral from posterior dorsolateral frontal cortex in memory processing. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[54]  A. Diamond Developmental Time Course in Human Infants and Infant Monkeys, and the Neural Bases of, Inhibitory Control in Reaching a , 1990, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[55]  R. Passingham,et al.  The nucleus accumbens in monkeys (Macaca fascicularis): I. The organization of behaviour , 1994, Behavioural Brain Research.

[56]  C. Marsden,et al.  Fronto-striatal cognitive deficits at different stages of Parkinson's disease. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[57]  T. Robbins,et al.  Planning and spatial working memory in Parkinson's disease. , 1988, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[58]  H. E. Rosvold,et al.  Localization of function within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the rhesus monkey. , 1970, Experimental neurology.

[59]  Richard Passingham,et al.  Delayed matching after selective prefrontal lesions in monkeys (Macaca mulatta) , 1975, Brain Research.

[60]  R. Passingham The frontal lobes and voluntary action , 1993 .

[61]  K. Brodmann Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Großhirnrinde : in ihren Prinzipien dargestellt auf Grund des Zellenbaues , 1985 .

[62]  T. Robbins,et al.  Cognitive functions and corticostriatal circuits: insights from Huntington's disease , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[63]  H. E. Rosvold,et al.  Cognitive deficit caused by regional depletion of dopamine in prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkey. , 1979, Science.

[64]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Dissociation of deficits on auditory tasks following partial prefrontal lesions in monkeys. , 1975, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.

[65]  J. Glowinski,et al.  Inhibitory influence of the mesocortical dopaminergic system on spontaneous activity or excitatory response induced from the thalamic mediodorsal nucleus in the rat medial prefrontal cortex , 1984, Brain Research.