Haptic communication between humans is tuned by the hard or soft mechanics of interaction

To move a hard table together, humans may coordinate by following the dominant partner’s motion [1–4], but this strategy is unsuitable for a soft mattress where the perceived forces are small. How do partners readily coordinate in such differing interaction dynamics? To address this, we investigated how pairs tracked a target using flexion-extension of their wrists, which were coupled by a hard, medium or soft virtual elastic band. Tracking performance monotonically increased with a stiffer band for the worse partner, who had higher tracking error, at the cost of the skilled partner’s muscular effort. This suggests that the worse partner followed the skilled one’s lead, but simulations show that the results are better explained by a model where partners share movement goals through the forces, whilst the coupling dynamics determine the capacity of communicable information. This model elucidates the versatile mechanism by which humans can coordinate during both hard and soft physical interactions to ensure maximum performance with minimal effort.

[1]  Martin Buss,et al.  Experimental analysis of dominance in haptic collaboration , 2009, RO-MAN 2009 - The 18th IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication.

[2]  Etienne Burdet,et al.  Facing the partner influences exchanges in force , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[3]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Joint action: bodies and minds moving together , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[4]  Kyle B. Reed,et al.  Physical Collaboration of Human-Human and Human-Robot Teams , 2008, IEEE Transactions on Haptics.

[5]  Cagatay Basdogan,et al.  The role of roles: Physical cooperation between humans and robots , 2012, Int. J. Robotics Res..

[6]  T. Başar,et al.  A New Approach to Linear Filtering and Prediction Problems , 2001 .

[7]  P. R. Laughlin,et al.  Demonstrability and social combination processes on mathematical intellective tasks. , 1986 .

[8]  Paul Evrard,et al.  Homotopy switching model for dyad haptic interaction in physical collaborative tasks , 2009, World Haptics 2009 - Third Joint EuroHaptics conference and Symposium on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environment and Teleoperator Systems.

[9]  R. Pfeifer,et al.  Self-Organization, Embodiment, and Biologically Inspired Robotics , 2007, Science.

[10]  Frans C T van der Helm,et al.  Sensory Weighting of Force and Position Feedback in Human Motor Control Tasks , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  Cagatay Basdogan,et al.  An experimental study on the role of touch in shared virtual environments , 2000, TCHI.

[12]  Martin Buss,et al.  Role determination in human-human interaction , 2009, World Haptics 2009 - Third Joint EuroHaptics conference and Symposium on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environment and Teleoperator Systems.

[13]  Etienne Burdet,et al.  Motion Plan Changes Predictably in Dyadic Reaching , 2016, PloS one.

[14]  A. Geurts,et al.  Definition dependent properties of the cortical silent period in upper-extremity muscles, a methodological study , 2014, Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation.

[15]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning , 2004, Nature.

[16]  Robrecht P R D van der Wel,et al.  Let the force be with us: dyads exploit haptic coupling for coordination. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  E. Burdet,et al.  A Framework to Describe, Analyze and Generate Interactive Motor Behaviors , 2012, PloS one.

[18]  Etienne Burdet,et al.  Hi5: A versatile dual-wrist device to study human-human interaction and bimanual control , 2011, 2011 IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems.

[19]  Reid Hastie,et al.  The robust beauty of majority rules in group decisions. , 2005, Psychological review.

[20]  Rolf Pfeifer,et al.  Morphological Computation - Connecting Brain, Body, and Environment , 2006, Australian Conference on Artificial Intelligence.

[21]  Sandra Hirche,et al.  Performance related energy exchange in haptic human-human interaction in a shared virtual object manipulation task , 2009, World Haptics 2009 - Third Joint EuroHaptics conference and Symposium on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environment and Teleoperator Systems.

[22]  Eric Feron,et al.  Human-human haptic collaboration in cyclical Fitts' tasks , 2005, 2005 IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems.

[23]  M. Kawato,et al.  Two is better than one: Physical interactions improve motor performance in humans , 2014, Scientific Reports.

[24]  Peter M. Vishton,et al.  Haptically Linked Dyads , 2006, Psychological science.

[25]  L. Ting,et al.  Perspectives on human-human sensorimotor interactions for the design of rehabilitation robots , 2014, Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation.

[26]  M. Ernst,et al.  Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.

[27]  Cagatay Basdogan,et al.  Haptic negotiation and role exchange for collaboration in virtual environments , 2010, 2010 IEEE Haptics Symposium.

[28]  E. Burdet,et al.  Interpersonal strategies for disturbance attenuation during a rhythmic joint motor action , 2015, Physiology & Behavior.

[29]  Etienne Burdet,et al.  Motor planning explains human behaviour in tasks with multiple solutions , 2013, Robotics Auton. Syst..

[30]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Physically interacting individuals estimate the partner’s goal to enhance their movements , 2017, Nature Human Behaviour.