The anthropologist as a primatologist. Mental journeys of a fieldworker

A Well-Travelled Discipline Experiencing destinations far from home is a dream that feeds the tourism industry. Those who explore foreign scenery, customs, food, nature, wildlife as professionals are called anthropologists. According to Greek etymology, the diversity of humans (anthropos) is the subject of their words and wisdom (logos). But anthropologists do not travel. They conduct ‘fieldwork’, a term that rings of wilderness. A German equivalent of the term has a truly poetic tinge to it: ‘Freilandforschung’, meaning ‘research in free land’. For long and intensive periods, far from home and family, these researchers are thoroughly immersed in ‘the other’, taking shelter with natives, learning their tongue, sharing food and rituals, partaking in strange ways of life and death while filling stacks of notebooks. Solid fieldwork is a core research method of anthropology. Once returned, these voyagers become objects of fascination themselves, exuding their strange experiences in lectures and monographs. Fieldwork marks anthropologists of good standing and mythical proportions can be attained if the quest was difficult. For example, ‘Argonauts of the Western Pacific’ (1922): who could have dreamed up a better title for that first major field study, which turned Bronislaw Malinowski (1884–1942) into an icon? Fieldwork can be gripping because of inherent surprises and unpredictability. The popularity of fictional anthropologists such as ‘Indiana Jones’ reflects this. Many would want to go on treasure hunts and tell heroic stories of survival. Admittedly, my portrayal of the anthropologist as a fieldworker is outdated. Often, anthropologists do not work in the field and those who do might be a far cry from the cliché of the lone explorer roughing it amongst natives. Contemporary research is less likely

[1]  V. Sommer,et al.  Subsistence Technology of Nigerian Chimpanzees , 2007, International Journal of Primatology.

[2]  M. Daly,et al.  Sex, evolution, and behavior , 1978 .

[3]  P. Bohannan Cultural Anthropology , 2020, Nature.

[4]  A. Whiten,et al.  CHARTING CULTURAL VARIATION IN CHIMPANZEES , 2001 .

[5]  K. Strier Faces in the Forest: The Endangered Muriqui Monkeys of Brazil , 1992 .

[6]  A. Whiten,et al.  Cultural panthropology , 2003 .

[7]  S. Hrdy The Woman That Never Evolved , 1999 .

[8]  T. Nagel Mortal Questions: What is it like to be a bat? , 2012 .

[9]  U. Reichard,et al.  Rethinking monogamy: the gibbon case , 2000 .

[10]  V. Sommer Infanticide among free-ranging langurs (Presbytis entellus) at jodhpur (Rajasthan/India): Recent observations and a reconsideration of hypotheses , 1987, Primates.

[11]  Rachel Nowak,et al.  Almost human. , 1999, New scientist.

[12]  Sommer,et al.  The Nigerian Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes vellerosus) at Gashaka: Two years of habituation efforts. - (Abstracts, 8. Kongress, Gesellschaft für Primatologie, 02-05 Oct 03, Leipzig). , 2003 .

[13]  Geoffrey Hall,et al.  Animal cognition , 1985, Nature.

[14]  E. Lowman,et al.  The shadow of a man. , 1967, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[15]  Paola Cavalieri,et al.  The Great ape project : equality beyond humanity , 1994 .

[16]  A Primate's Memoir , 2001 .

[17]  Eric Bonabeau The handicap principle , 1998, Complex..

[18]  V. Sommer,et al.  Leaf-swallowing in Nigerian chimpanzees: evidence for assumed self-medication , 2006, Primates.

[19]  G. Schaller THE MOUNTAIN GORILLA - Ecology and Behavior , 1963 .

[20]  Vernon Reynolds,et al.  Primate Visions: Gender, Race, and Nature in the World of Modern Science. , 1991 .