The representational capacity of the distributed encoding of information provided by populations of neurons in primate temporal visual cortex

Abstract It has been shown that it is possible to read, from the firing rates of just a small population of neurons, the code that is used in the macaque temporal lobe visual cortex to distinguish between different faces being looked at. To analyse the information provided by populations of single neurons in the primate temporal cortical visual areas, the responses of a population of 14 neurons to 20 visual stimuli were analysed in a macaque performing a visual fixation task. The population of neurons analysed responded primarily to faces, and the stimuli utilised were all human and monkey faces. Each neuron had its own response profile to the different members of the stimulus set. The mean response of each neuron to each stimulus in the set was calculated from a fraction of the ten trials of data available for every stimulus. From the remaining data, it was possible to calculate, for any population response vector, the relative likelihoods that it had been elicited by each of the stimuli in the set. By comparison with the stimuli actually shown, the mean percentage correct identification was computed and also the mean information about the stimuli, in bits, that the population of neurons carried on a single trial. When the decoding algorithm used for this calculation approximated an optimal, Bayesian estimate of the relative likelihoods, the percentage correct increased from 14% correct (chance was 5% correct) with one neuron to 67% with 14 neurons. The information conveyed by the population of neurons increased approximately linearly from 0.33 bits with one neuron to 2.77 bits with 14 neurons. This leads to the important conclusion that the number of stimuli that can be encoded by a population of neurons in this part of the visual system increases approximately exponentially as the number of cells in the sample increases (in that the log of the number of stimuli increases almost linearly). This is in contrast to a local encoding scheme (of ”grandmother” cells), in which the number of stimuli encoded increases linearly with the number of cells in the sample. Thus one of the potentially important properties of distributed representations, an exponential increase in the number of stimuli that can be represented, has been demonstrated in the brain with this population of neurons. When the algorithm used for estimating stimulus likelihood was as simple as could be easily implemented by neurons receiving the population’s output (based on just the dot product between the population response vector and each mean response vector), it was still found that the 14-neuron population produced 66% correct guesses and conveyed 2.30 bits of information, or 83% of the information that could be extracted with the nearly optimal procedure. It was also shown that, although there was some redundancy in the representation (with each neuron contributing to the information carried by the whole population 60% of the information it carried alone, rather than 100%), this is due to the fact that the number of stimuli in the set was limited (it was 20). The data are consistent with minimal redundancy for sufficiently large and diverse sets of stimuli. The implication for brain connectivity of the distributed encoding scheme, which was demonstrated here in the case of faces, is that a neuron can receive a great deal of information about what is encoded by a large population of neurons if it is able to receive its inputs from a random subset of these neurons, even of limited numbers (e.g. hundreds).

[1]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Coding visual images of objects in the inferotemporal cortex of the macaque monkey. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  E. Rolls,et al.  Selectivity between faces in the responses of a population of neurons in the cortex in the superior temporal sulcus of the monkey , 1985, Brain Research.

[3]  Rolls Et Neurons in the cortex of the temporal lobe and in the amygdala of the monkey with responses selective for faces. , 1984 .

[4]  E. Rolls Neural organization of higher visual functions , 1991, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[5]  M. Tovée,et al.  Processing speed in the cerebral cortex and the neurophysiology of visual masking , 1994, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[6]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Visual processing in monkey extrastriate cortex. , 1987, Annual review of neuroscience.

[7]  E. Rolls,et al.  Functional subdivisions of the temporal lobe neocortex , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[8]  Stefano Panzeri,et al.  How Well Can We Estimate the Information Carried in Neuronal Responses from Limited Samples? , 1997, Neural Computation.

[9]  Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al.  The Computational Brain , 1996, Artif. Intell..

[10]  E. Rolls,et al.  Face and voice expression identification in patients with emotional and behavioural changes following ventral frontal lobe damage , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  L. Weiskrantz,et al.  Impairments of visual object transforms in monkeys. , 1984, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[12]  Ehud Zohary,et al.  Correlated neuronal discharge rate and its implications for psychophysical performance , 1994, Nature.

[13]  W. Singer,et al.  Oscillatory responses in cat visual cortex exhibit inter-columnar synchronization which reflects global stimulus properties , 1989, Nature.

[14]  R. Desimone,et al.  Stimulus-selective properties of inferior temporal neurons in the macaque , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[15]  E. Rolls Brain mechanisms for invariant visual recognition and learning , 1994, Behavioural Processes.

[16]  R. Desimone,et al.  Visual areas in the temporal cortex of the macaque , 1979, Brain Research.

[17]  Edmund T. Rolls,et al.  Neurophysiology and functions of the primate amygdala. , 1992 .

[18]  E. Rolls Functions of neuronal networks in the hippocampus and neocortex in memory , 1989 .

[19]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Primate motor cortex and free arm movements to visual targets in three- dimensional space. II. Coding of the direction of movement by a neuronal population , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[20]  Treves,et al.  Graded-response neurons and information encodings in autoassociative memories. , 1990, Physical review. A, Atomic, molecular, and optical physics.

[21]  R. Desimone,et al.  Inferior Temporal Cortex and Pattern Recognition , 1985 .

[22]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Distributed Representations , 1986, The Philosophy of Artificial Intelligence.

[23]  D. Pandya,et al.  Afferent cortical connections and architectonics of the superior temporal sulcus and surrounding cortex in the rhesus monkey , 1978, Brain Research.

[24]  Edmund T. Rolls,et al.  The relative advantages of sparse versus distributed encoding for associative neuronal networks in the brain , 1990 .

[25]  A Treves,et al.  On the perceptual structure of face space. , 1997, Bio Systems.

[26]  Stefano Panzeri,et al.  The Upward Bias in Measures of Information Derived from Limited Data Samples , 1995, Neural Computation.

[27]  N Suga,et al.  Principles of auditory information-processing derived from neuroethology. , 1989, The Journal of experimental biology.

[28]  R. Desimone,et al.  Visual properties of neurons in a polysensory area in superior temporal sulcus of the macaque. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  E. Rolls Learning mechanisms in the temporal lobe visual cortex , 1995, Behavioural Brain Research.

[30]  C. Gross,et al.  Neural ensemble coding in inferior temporal cortex. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  E. Rolls,et al.  Computational analysis of the role of the hippocampus in memory , 1994, Hippocampus.

[32]  M. Tovée,et al.  Representational capacity of face coding in monkeys. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[33]  Edmund T. Rolls,et al.  What determines the capacity of autoassociative memories in the brain? Network , 1991 .

[34]  M. Tovée,et al.  Information encoding and the responses of single neurons in the primate temporal visual cortex. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  E T Rolls,et al.  Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying face processing within and beyond the temporal cortical visual areas. , 1992, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[36]  Stefano Panzeri,et al.  Analytical estimates of limited sampling biases in different information measures. , 1996, Network.

[37]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Organization of visual inputs to the inferior temporal and posterior parietal cortex in macaques , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[38]  K Tanaka,et al.  Neuronal mechanisms of object recognition. , 1993, Science.

[39]  P. Földiák,et al.  The ‘Ideal Homunculus’: Statistical Inference from Neural Population Responses , 1993 .

[40]  William Bialek,et al.  Reading a Neural Code , 1991, NIPS.

[41]  TJ Gawne,et al.  How independent are the messages carried by adjacent inferior temporal cortical neurons? , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[42]  M. Tovée,et al.  Information encoding in short firing rate epochs by single neurons in the primate temporal visual cortex , 1995 .

[43]  W. Singer,et al.  Temporal coding in the visual cortex: new vistas on integration in the nervous system , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[44]  E T Rolls,et al.  Sparseness of the neuronal representation of stimuli in the primate temporal visual cortex. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[45]  B L McNaughton,et al.  Dynamics of the hippocampal ensemble code for space. , 1993, Science.

[46]  R. Desimone Face-Selective Cells in the Temporal Cortex of Monkeys , 1991, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[47]  Naftali Tishby,et al.  Cortical activity flips among quasi-stationary states. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[48]  E. Rolls,et al.  Emotion-related learning in patients with social and emotional changes associated with frontal lobe damage. , 1994, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[49]  E. Rolls Neurons in the cortex of the temporal lobe and in the amygdala of the monkey with responses selective for faces. , 1984, Human neurobiology.