Embodied Space in Early Blind Individuals

The impact of sensory experience during early life on space perception and control of action has only been scarcely studied. The visual system typically provides the more accurate and reliable spatial information of our surrounding and is then usually considered as the frontrunner sense when spatial processing is at play. The study of visually deprived individual therefore offers a unique opportunity to investigate the role that vision plays in shaping how we process our surrounding space. However, aside quantitative differences between sighted and blind people in their perceptual skills, visual deprivation may also result in qualitatively different ways of processing non-visual information (Eimer, 2004).

[1]  C. Spence,et al.  Confusing the mind by crossing the hands. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[2]  A. Eardley,et al.  Event‐related potential evidence for the use of external coordinates in the preparation of tactile attention by the early blind , 2011, The European journal of neuroscience.

[3]  M. Paré,et al.  Early-blind human subjects localize sound sources better than sighted subjects , 1998, Nature.

[4]  C. Spence,et al.  Visual Capture of Touch: Out-of-the-Body Experiences With Rubber Gloves , 2000, Psychological science.

[5]  D. H. Warren,et al.  Sensory conflict in judgments of spatial direction , 1969 .

[6]  G. Vandewalle,et al.  Functional specialization for auditory–spatial processing in the occipital cortex of congenitally blind humans , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[7]  Olivier Collignon,et al.  Improved selective and divided spatial attention in early blind subjects , 2006, Brain Research.

[8]  E. Sampaio,et al.  Tactile and visual bisection tasks by sighted and blind children , 1995 .

[9]  C. Spence,et al.  Developmental vision determines the reference frame for the multisensory control of action , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[10]  S. Soto-Faraco,et al.  Changing Reference Frames during the Encoding of Tactile Events , 2008, Current Biology.

[11]  M. McCourt,et al.  Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  D. Goldreich,et al.  Tactile Spatial Acuity Enhancement in Blindness: Evidence for Experience-Dependent Mechanisms , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  F. Rösler,et al.  Altered auditory-tactile interactions in congenitally blind humans: an event-related potential study , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  Brigitte Röder,et al.  Common Anatomical and External Coding for Hands and Feet in Tactile Attention: Evidence from Event-related Potentials , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[15]  Matthijs L. Noordzij,et al.  The influence of visual experience on the ability to form spatial mental models based on route and survey descriptions , 2006, Cognition.

[16]  R. Zatorre,et al.  A Functional Neuroimaging Study of Sound Localization: Visual Cortex Activity Predicts Performance in Early-Blind Individuals , 2005, PLoS biology.

[17]  M. Eimer Multisensory Integration: How Visual Experience Shapes Spatial Perception , 2004, Current Biology.

[18]  M. Lassonde,et al.  Cross-modal plasticity for the spatial processing of sounds in visually deprived subjects , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  S. Soto-Faraco,et al.  Somatosensory saccades reveal the timing of tactile spatial remapping , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  O. Collignon,et al.  Further evidence that congenitally blind participants react faster to auditory and tactile spatial targets. , 2009, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[21]  D. Burr,et al.  The Ventriloquist Effect Results from Near-Optimal Bimodal Integration , 2004, Current Biology.

[22]  I Rock,et al.  Form perception without a retinal image. , 1969, The American journal of psychology.

[23]  S. Kitazawa Where conscious sensation takes place , 2002, Consciousness and Cognition.

[24]  Ehud Zohary,et al.  Is That Near My Hand? Multisensory Representation of Peripersonal Space in Human Intraparietal Sulcus , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  John L. Bradshaw,et al.  Tactual-kinesthetic matching of horizontal extents by the long-term blind: Absence or reversal of normal left-side underestimation , 1986, Neuropsychologia.

[26]  S. Kitazawa,et al.  Reversal of subjective temporal order due to arm crossing , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[27]  C. Spence,et al.  Infants lost in (peripersonal) space? , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[28]  D. H. Warren,et al.  Visual-proprioceptive interaction under large amounts of conflict. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.

[29]  Cesare Cornoldi,et al.  Spatial memory and integration processes in congenital blindness , 2004, Neuroreport.

[30]  Franco Lepore,et al.  Embodied numbers: The role of vision in the development of number–space interactions , 2013, Cortex.

[31]  M. Davare,et al.  Reorganisation of the Right Occipito-Parietal Stream for Auditory Spatial Processing in Early Blind Humans. A Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Study , 2009, Brain Topography.

[32]  P. Haggard,et al.  The Posterior Parietal Cortex Remaps Touch into External Space , 2010, Current Biology.

[33]  C. Spence,et al.  Early Vision Impairs Tactile Perception in the Blind , 2004, Current Biology.

[34]  Risto Näätänen,et al.  Faster reaction times in the blind than sighted during bimodal divided attention , 1997 .

[35]  C. Spence,et al.  Spatial coordinate systems for tactile spatial attention depend on developmental vision: evidence from event‐related potentials in sighted and congenitally blind adult humans , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.

[36]  S. Soto-Faraco,et al.  Tactile remapping beyond space , 2010, The European journal of neuroscience.

[37]  Seymour Axelrod,et al.  Effects of early blindness : performance of blind and sighted children on tactile and auditory tasks , 1959 .