Testosterone, cortisol, and women's competition.

Abstract Hormone (testosterone, cortisol)–behavior relationships have been extensively studied among male competitors, and far less so among female competitors. To address this gap, we studied members of a nationally recognized college women's rugby team. Seventeen players (ages 18–22 years) provided saliva samples 24 h before, 20 min prior to, and immediately after five league matches. Subjects self-reported aggressiveness, team bonding, pregame mental state, postgame performance evaluation, and whether the opponent was more or less challenging than expected. Results revealed that both testosterone and cortisol levels increased in anticipation of the matches. Postgame levels of both hormones were higher than pregame levels. The pregame rise in testosterone was associated with team bonding, aggressiveness, and being focused, but was unrelated to perceptions of the opponent's skill. Testosterone change during the game was unrelated to winning or losing, evaluations of personal performance, or perceptions of the opponent's threat. Game changes in cortisol were positively related to player evaluations of whether the opponent was more of a challenge than expected, and negatively related to losing. These results are compared with hormone–behavior patterns found among male competitors and are interpreted within a recent theory of sex differences in response to challenges.

[1]  W. Gogel,et al.  Effects of infused testosterone on mental performances and serum LH. , 1971, The Journal of clinical endocrinology and metabolism.

[2]  P. Bernhardt,et al.  Testosterone changes during vicarious experiences of winning and losing among fans at sporting events , 1998, Physiology & Behavior.

[3]  Michael Elias Serum cortisol, testosterone, and testosterone-binding globulin responses to competitive fighting in human males. , 1981 .

[4]  E. Filaire,et al.  Anxiety, Hormonal Responses, and Coping During a Judo Competition , 2001 .

[5]  Alan Booth,et al.  Salivary Testosterone Determination in Studies of Child Health and Development , 1999, Hormones and Behavior.

[6]  D. Broverman,et al.  Effects of testosterone infusions upon EEGs of normal male adults. , 1971, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[7]  M. Zuckerman,et al.  Disinhibitory sensation seeking, personality and gonadal hormones , 1980 .

[8]  K. Mccaul,et al.  Hormonal response to competition in human males , 1989 .

[9]  A. Salvador,et al.  Testosterone and cortisol responses to competitive fighting in human males: A pilot study , 1987 .

[10]  Allan Mazur,et al.  Testosterone, and winning and losing in human competition , 1989, Hormones and Behavior.

[11]  John A Updegraff,et al.  Biobehavioral responses to stress in females: tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight. , 2000, Psychological review.

[12]  Allan Mazur,et al.  Sex Difference in Testosterone Response to a Video Game Contest , 1997 .

[13]  E. González-Bono,et al.  Testosterone, Cortisol, and Mood in a Sports Team Competition , 1999, Hormones and Behavior.

[14]  Theodore A. Lamb,et al.  Testosterone, status, and mood in human males , 1980, Hormones and Behavior.

[15]  E. Korn,et al.  Regression analysis with clustered data. , 1994, Statistics in medicine.

[16]  M. Gunnar,et al.  Assessing salivary cortisol in studies of child development. , 1998, Child development.

[17]  J. M. Dabbs,et al.  Testosterone and chess competition. , 1992 .

[18]  Randy J. Nelson,et al.  An Introduction to Behavioral Endocrinology , 2000 .

[19]  G. Jm [Statistics and medicine]. , 1987, Medicina clinica.