TPJ-M1 interaction in the control of shared representations: New insights from tDCS and TMS combined

Abstract There is extensive evidence that perceived and internally planned actions have a common representational basis: action observation can induce an automatic tendency to imitate others. If perceived and executed action, however, are based on shared representations, the question arises how we can distinguish self‐related and other‐related representations. It has been suggested that the control of shared representations involves a neural network centered on the temporo‐parietal junction (TPJ). However, the specific role of the TPJ in controlling shared representations is still not clear. In a conflict situation where participants have to execute action A while observing action B, the TPJ might either facilitate the relevant action A or inhibit the irrelevant action B (mirror response). In the present study, we used transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS) to condition neural activity in the right temporo‐parietal junction (TPJ). We then analyzed the corticospinal output as indexed by motor‐evoked potentials (MEPs) induced by single‐pulse TMS (spTMS) of the left primary motor cortex (M1) during action observation in the context of a conflict task. Results showed that tDCS‐mediated increased control did not entail the attenuation of the task‐irrelevant response activation: the effect of motor mirroring was not suppressed or reduced. Rather, facilitating TPJ activity via anodal tDCS selectively enhanced the instructed motor plan (self‐related representation). This outcome supports the idea that TPJ plays a critical role in detecting the mismatch between self‐related and other‐related representations and is at work to enhance task‐relevant representations. HighlightsWe investigated the functional interaction between TPJ and M1 in the control of shared representations.TPJ stimulation enhances task‐relevant motor representations as indexed by MEPs analysis.

[1]  R Saxe,et al.  People thinking about thinking people The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind” , 2003, NeuroImage.

[2]  M. Hallett,et al.  Modeling the current distribution during transcranial direct current stimulation , 2006, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[3]  Y. Paulignan,et al.  An Interference Effect of Observed Biological Movement on Action , 2003, Current Biology.

[4]  Cleotilde Gonzalez,et al.  Dissociation of S-R compatibility and Simon effects with mixed tasks and mappings. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[5]  C. Keysers,et al.  Evidence for mirror systems in emotions , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[6]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Abnormalities in the awareness of action , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[7]  C. Heyes Automatic imitation. , 2011, Psychological bulletin.

[8]  Jakob Hohwy,et al.  Can neuroscience explain consciousness , 2004 .

[9]  Patrick Ragert,et al.  The medial frontal cortex mediates self-other discrimination in the joint Simon task: A tDCS study , 2016 .

[10]  M. Brass,et al.  The inhibition of imitative and overlearned responses: a functional double dissociation , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  Mi Young Lee,et al.  The effect of transcranial direct current stimulation on the cortical activation by motor task in the human brain: An fMRI study , 2009, Neuroscience Letters.

[12]  M. Koslowsky,et al.  tDCS polarity effects in motor and cognitive domains: a meta-analytical review , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  S. Sato,et al.  Safety and cognitive effect of frontal DC brain polarization in healthy individuals , 2005, Neurology.

[14]  Zoubin Ghahramani,et al.  Computational principles of movement neuroscience , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[15]  A. Newen,et al.  Beyond the comparator model: A multifactorial two-step account of agency , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.

[16]  A. Osman,et al.  Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.

[17]  Antonia F. de C. Hamilton,et al.  The mirror neuron system contributes to social responding , 2013, Cortex.

[18]  G. Bird,et al.  Transcranial Current Stimulation of the Temporoparietal Junction Improves Lie Detection , 2015, Current Biology.

[19]  Luigi Cattaneo,et al.  Bottom-up and top-down visuomotor responses to action observation. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[20]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[21]  Caroline Catmur,et al.  Sensorimotor Learning Configures the Human Mirror System , 2007, Current Biology.

[22]  M. Nitsche,et al.  Sustained excitability elevations induced by transcranial DC motor cortex stimulation in humans , 2001, Neurology.

[23]  M. Brass,et al.  Imitation: is cognitive neuroscience solving the correspondence problem? , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[24]  S. Rossi,et al.  Safety, ethical considerations, and application guidelines for the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in clinical practice and research , 2009, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[25]  M. Jeannerod The 25th Bartlett Lecture , 1999 .

[26]  Cristina Becchio,et al.  Timecourse of mirror and counter-mirror effects measured with transcranial magnetic stimulation , 2013, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[27]  Eliot Hazeltine,et al.  Dissociable Contributions of Prefrontal and Parietal Cortices to Response Selection , 2002, NeuroImage.

[28]  Xun Liu,et al.  Common and distinct neural substrates of attentional control in an integrated Simon and spatial Stroop task as assessed by event-related fMRI , 2004, NeuroImage.

[29]  Simone Rossi,et al.  Cortico-Cortical Connectivity between Right Parietal and Bilateral Primary Motor Cortices during Imagined and Observed Actions: A Combined TMS/tDCS Study , 2011, Front. Neural Circuits.

[30]  Richard Ramsey,et al.  The Control of Mimicry by Eye Contact Is Mediated by Medial Prefrontal Cortex , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[32]  J. Decety,et al.  Effect of subjective perspective taking during simulation of action: a PET investigation of agency , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[33]  Geoffrey Bird,et al.  Enhancing Social Ability by Stimulating Right Temporoparietal Junction Tdcs of Right Tpj , 2022 .

[34]  W. Prinz,et al.  Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. , 2001, Acta psychologica.

[35]  C. Frith,et al.  Experiencing Oneself vs Another Person as Being the Cause of an Action: The Neural Correlates of the Experience of Agency , 2002, NeuroImage.

[36]  Neil G. Muggleton,et al.  Modulating inhibitory control with direct current stimulation of the superior medial frontal cortex , 2011, NeuroImage.

[37]  Marcel Brass,et al.  Was it me or was it you? How the sense of agency originates from ideomotor learning revealed by fMRI , 2009, NeuroImage.

[38]  M. Brass,et al.  Inhibition of imitative behaviour and social cognition , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[39]  Luciano Fadiga,et al.  Hand action preparation influences the responses to hand pictures , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  J. Perner,et al.  Common brain areas engaged in false belief reasoning and visual perspective taking: a meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[41]  C. Keysers,et al.  Social Neuroscience: Mirror Neurons Recorded in Humans , 2010, Current Biology.

[42]  Ingrid R. Olson,et al.  A selective working memory impairment after transcranial direct current stimulation to the right parietal lobe , 2010, Neuroscience Letters.

[43]  C. Frith,et al.  Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[44]  F. Overwalle Social cognition and the brain: a meta-analysis. , 2009 .

[45]  Lara Bardi,et al.  Eliminating mirror responses by instructions , 2015, Cortex.

[46]  Michael Erb,et al.  The neural correlates of perceiving one's own movements , 2003, NeuroImage.

[47]  Roberta Sellaro,et al.  The stimulated social brain: effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on social cognition , 2016, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[48]  Geoffrey Bird,et al.  Functional lateralization of temporoparietal junction – imitation inhibition, visual perspective‐taking and theory of mind , 2015, The European journal of neuroscience.

[49]  Baptist Liefooghe,et al.  Eliminating the Simon effect by instruction. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[50]  Pablo Celnik,et al.  Brain polarization enhances the formation and retention of motor memories. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[51]  C. Keysers,et al.  The Observation and Execution of Actions Share Motor and Somatosensory Voxels in all Tested Subjects: Single-Subject Analyses of Unsmoothed fMRI Data , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[52]  Luigi Cattaneo,et al.  Early and late motor responses to action observation. , 2013, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[53]  J. Decety,et al.  Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation, observation, and verb generation of actions: A meta‐analysis , 2001, Human brain mapping.

[54]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[55]  M. Hallett,et al.  The neural processes underlying self-agency. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[56]  C. Frith,et al.  Interacting minds--a biological basis. , 1999, Science.

[57]  P. Matthews,et al.  Polarity-Sensitive Modulation of Cortical Neurotransmitters by Transcranial Stimulation , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[58]  C. Frith,et al.  The Neural Basis of Mentalizing , 2006, Neuron.

[59]  Caroline Catmur,et al.  The Role of the Right Temporoparietal Junction in the Control of Imitation , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[60]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Applications of transcranial direct current stimulation for understanding brain function , 2014, Trends in Neurosciences.

[61]  T. Chartrand,et al.  The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[62]  W. Prinz,et al.  Compatibility between Observed and Executed Finger Movements: Comparing Symbolic, Spatial, and Imitative Cues , 2000, Brain and Cognition.

[63]  C. Heyes,et al.  Mirror neurons: from origin to function. , 2014, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[64]  J. Perner,et al.  Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews Fractionating Theory of Mind: a Meta-analysis of Functional Brain Imaging Studies , 2022 .

[65]  Marc Jeannerod,et al.  Modulating the experience of agency: a positron emission tomography study , 2003, NeuroImage.

[66]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Studying the neurobiology of social interaction with transcranial direct current stimulation--the example of punishing unfairness. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[67]  M. Jeannerod TO ACT OR NOT TO ACT : PERSPECTIVES ON THE REPRESENTATION OF ACTIONS , 1999 .

[68]  S. Hurley The shared circuits model (SCM): how control, mirroring, and simulation can enable imitation, deliberation, and mindreading. , 2008, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[69]  Umberto Castiello,et al.  When emulation becomes reciprocity. , 2013, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[70]  Sarah-Jayne Blakemore,et al.  The role of motor contagion in the prediction of action , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[71]  Jennifer L. Cook,et al.  Task-relevance dependent gradients in medial prefrontal and temporoparietal cortices suggest solutions to paradoxes concerning self/other control , 2014, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[72]  M. Nitsche,et al.  Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation , 2000, The Journal of physiology.

[73]  Michela Romani,et al.  Motor facilitation during action observation: topographic mapping of the target muscle and influence of the onlooker's posture , 2006, The European journal of neuroscience.