Do evolutionary processes minimize expected losses?

Evolution by variation and natural selection is often viewed as an optimization process that favors those organisms which are best adapted to their environment. This leaves open the issue of how to measure adaptation and what criterion is implied for optimization. This problem has been framed and analysed mathematically under the assumption that individuals compete to minimize expected losses across a series of decisions (e.g. choice of behavior), where each decision offers a stochastic payoff. But the fact that a particular analysis is tractable for a specified criterion does not imply the fidelity of that criterion. Computer simulations involving a version of the k -armed bandit problem can address the veracity of the hypothesis that individuals are selected to minimize expected losses. The results offered here do not support this hypothesis.

[1]  Richard F. Green,et al.  Bayesian birds: A simple example of Oaten's stochastic model of optimal foraging , 1980 .

[2]  Günter Rudolph,et al.  Reflections on Bandit Problems and Selection Methods in Uncertain Environments , 1998, ICGA.

[3]  P. Taylor,et al.  Test of optimal sampling by foraging great tits , 1978 .

[4]  G. Unter Rudolph,et al.  Re ections on Bandit Problems and Selection Methods in Uncertain Environments , 1997 .

[5]  Hans-Georg Beyer,et al.  Toward a Theory of Evolution Strategies: On the Benefits of Sex the (/, ) Theory , 1995, Evolutionary Computation.

[6]  B. Arnold,et al.  A first course in order statistics , 1994 .

[7]  John Holland,et al.  Adaptation in Natural and Artificial Sys-tems: An Introductory Analysis with Applications to Biology , 1975 .

[8]  John E. R. Staddon,et al.  Optima for animals , 1982 .

[9]  R. Meldola Sexual Selection , 1871, Nature.

[10]  T. Caraco,et al.  An empirical demonstration of risk-sensitive foraging preferences , 1980, Animal Behaviour.

[11]  Robert M. Seyfarth,et al.  The reproductive success of vervet monkeys , 1988 .

[12]  Alasdair I. Houston,et al.  Risk-sensitive foraging: A review of the theory , 1992 .

[13]  S. N. Tibbo,et al.  An Investigation of Herring (Clupea harengus L.) Spawning Using Free-Diving Techniques , 1963 .

[14]  David H. Wolpert,et al.  Bandit problems and the exploration/exploitation tradeoff , 1998, IEEE Trans. Evol. Comput..

[15]  E. Mayr Toward a new philosophy of biology: observations of an evolutionist , 1988 .

[16]  J. Blaxter,et al.  The Biology of the Clupeoid Fishes , 1982 .

[17]  J M Smith,et al.  Evolution and the theory of games , 1976 .

[18]  A. Kacelnik,et al.  Starlings’ preferences for predictable and unpredictable delays to food , 1997, Animal Behaviour.

[19]  D. Wood‐Gush Aggression and sexual activity in the brown leghorn cock , 1957 .

[20]  J. Gamble,et al.  The Early Life History of Fish , 1974, Springer Berlin Heidelberg.

[21]  E. Houde The Early Life History of Fish. , 1975 .

[22]  J. Goodall,et al.  Patterns of predation by chimpanzees on red colobus monkeys in Gombe National Park, 1982-1991. , 1994, American journal of physical anthropology.

[23]  C. Boesch The Effects of Leopard Predation On Grouping Patterns in Forest Chimpanzees , 1991 .

[24]  D. Farnsworth A First Course in Order Statistics , 1993 .

[25]  Roger Hewitt,et al.  THE VALUE OF PATTERN IN THE DISTRIBUTION OF YOUNG FISH , 1981 .

[26]  T. Caraco,et al.  Risk-sensitivity: ambient temperature affects foraging choice , 1990, Animal Behaviour.

[27]  John H. Holland,et al.  Adaptation in Natural and Artificial Systems: An Introductory Analysis with Applications to Biology, Control, and Artificial Intelligence , 1992 .