Vocal signalling of the great crested flycatcher, Myiarchus crinitus (Aves, Tyrannidae)

The singing and related vocalizing of great crested flycatchers, Myiarchus crinitus (Aves, Tyrannidae), included at least 12 different vocalizations. Each vocalization correlated with (and thus made information available about) a distinctive range of behaviour, from unassertive to more actively responsive, provocative or even confrontational. Four were used by individuals primarily when alone and not initiating close social encounters. The eight other vocalizations were uttered primarily in close encounters: one in preliminaries to such events, two predominantly in encounters with opponents, another in close associating of mates, two in duetting, and two infrequently, mostly in confrontations. The division of the repertoire into sets of vocalizations providing information about either low or elevated probabilities that a signaller will undertake the behavioural initiatives that precede, shape or sustain social interactions parallels a division found in other recently studied passerines. Such information is pertinent to individuals' decisions to forego or start close interactions with, for instance, competitors or mates. The information should be useful to a great many species, not just birds, but also mammals and various other animals — species in which individuals keep each other informed by singing when they are apart.

[1]  W. J. Smith,et al.  Playback Interactions with Great Crested Flycatchers, Myiarchus crinitus (Aves, Tyrannidae) , 2010 .

[2]  Derek A. Roff,et al.  Song Repertoires of American Warblers (Parulinae): Honest Advertising or Assessment? , 2010 .

[3]  A. Smith,et al.  Information about behaviour provided by Louisiana waterthrush,Seiurus motacilla (Parulinae), songs , 1996, Animal Behaviour.

[4]  W. John Smith,et al.  Singing is based on two markedly different kinds of signaling , 1991 .

[5]  F. Colmenares Greeting behaviour between male baboons: oestrous females, rivalry and negotiation , 1991, Animal Behaviour.

[6]  Torben Dabelsteen,et al.  Song and information about aggressive responses of blackbirds, Turdus merula: evidence from interactive playback experiments with territory owners , 1990, Animal Behaviour.

[7]  W. J. Smith Patterned daytime singing of the eastern wood-pewee, Contopus virens , 1988, Animal Behaviour.

[8]  J. Serpell Duetting in birds and primates: A question of function , 1981, Animal Behaviour.

[9]  J. Fitzpatrick,et al.  Foraging Behavior of Neotropical Tyrant Flycatchers , 1980 .

[10]  Thorpe Duetting and antiphonal song in birds; its extent and significance , 1972 .

[11]  R. Robertson,et al.  Mate guarding tactics used by Great Crested Flycatchers , 1995 .

[12]  R. H. Wiley,et al.  Use of Two Singing Modes By Hooded Warblers as Adaptations for Signalling , 1994 .

[13]  W. J. Smith,et al.  Behavioral Information Provided By Two Song Forms of the Eastern Kingbird, T. Tyrannus , 1992 .

[14]  Eugene S. Morton,et al.  Predictions From the Ranging Hypothesis for the Evolution of Long Distance Signals in Birds , 1986 .

[15]  R. Hinde Chapter 5 – Expression and Negotiation , 1985 .

[16]  Wendy Smith,et al.  Chapter 3 – Consistency and Change in Communication , 1985 .

[17]  R. Michael Observations Upon the Sexual Behaviour of the Domestic Cat (Felis Catus L.) Under Laboratory Conditions , 1961 .

[18]  M. Nice Notes on the Twilight Songs of the Scissor-Tailed and Crested Flycatchers , 1931 .