Cognitive procedural learning in patients with fronto-striatal lesions.

Previous studies on the role of prefronto-striatal loop systems for cognitive procedural learning (PL) brought inconsistent results. To examine whether the integrity of the dorsolateral prefrontal loop is indispensable for normal cognitive PL, we examined the acquisition of cognitive skills in 35 patients with focal prefrontal lesions, focal caudate lesions, and Huntington's Disease (HD), and compared it with a control group. To examine the potential role of the processing demands made by the cognitive tasks, a set of tasks was applied whose acquisition places demand either on reasoning and problem solving, or on the establishment of fast and repetitive processing routines. The Pursuit Rotor task was also studied with the aim to re-examine earlier findings of a functional segregation of motor and complex prefronto-striatal loops. Deficits of cognitive and motor PL were found to be limited to certain tasks and groups. PL of one task, which demanded rapid, repetitive processing of visuo-spatial stimuli, was impaired in all patient groups. PL of two problem-solving tasks was impaired in patients with focal and degenerative caudate lesions only. None of the groups was impaired at PL of Mirror Reading, another task demanding rapid and repetitive visuo-spatial processing, and none was impaired in a fifth task of language skill. Deficits of motor learning were only observed in patients in whom the motor loop was affected. These results suggest that the dorsolateral prefronto-striatal loop is involved in the establishment of cognitive processing routines. The comparison of patients with and without caudate lesions suggests that disconnection of this circuit is critical. However, the normal acquisition of two cognitive tasks even in this group indicates that the integrity of the dorsolateral prefrontal loop is not mandatory for normal cognitive PL. We discuss these dissociations with regard to the demand profiles of the applied tasks and the locations of the lesions.

[1]  R. Dolan,et al.  Neural systems engaged by planning: a PET study of the Tower of London task , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  C Caltagirone,et al.  Implicit memory in parkinsonian patients: evidence for deficient skill learning. , 1996, European neurology.

[3]  R. S. Williams,et al.  Morphometric analysis of the prefrontal cortex in Huntington's disease , 1991, Neurology.

[4]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Basal ganglia-thalamocortical circuits: parallel substrates for motor, oculomotor, "prefrontal" and "limbic" functions. , 1990, Progress in brain research.

[5]  A. Baddeley,et al.  What can amnesic patients learn? , 1976, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  Klaus Schmidtke,et al.  Cognitive Procedural Learning in Amnesia , 1996, Brain and Cognition.

[7]  O. Koenig,et al.  Procedural learning in Parkinson’s disease: Intact and impaired cognitive components , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  S. Petersen,et al.  Changes in brain activity during motor learning measured with PET: effects of hand of performance and practice. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  J. Saint-Cyr,et al.  Procedural learning and neostriatal dysfunction in man. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[10]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Functional architecture of basal ganglia circuits: neural substrates of parallel processing , 1990, Trends in Neurosciences.

[11]  J. Saint-Cyr,et al.  Behavior and the basal ganglia. , 1995, Advances in neurology.

[12]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Functional anatomy of human procedural learning determined with regional cerebral blood flow and PET , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[13]  L R Squire,et al.  Intact learning of artificial grammars and intact category learning by patients with Parkinson's disease. , 1999, Behavioral neuroscience.

[14]  Apostolos P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Cortico-basal ganglia relations and coding of motor performance , 1983 .

[15]  J. Vonsattel,et al.  Morphometric Demonstration of Atrophic Changes in the Cerebral Cortex, White Matter, and Neostriatum in Huntington's Disease , 1988, Journal of neuropathology and experimental neurology.

[16]  C. R. Kennedy,et al.  Advances in neurology. , 1996, Archives of disease in childhood.

[17]  J. Becker,et al.  Dissociations between skill learning and verbal recognition in amnesia and dementia. , 1984, Archives of neurology.

[18]  J. Saint-Cyr,et al.  The mobilization of procedural learning: The "key signature" of the basal ganglia. , 1992 .

[19]  A. Dagher,et al.  Mapping the network for planning: a correlational PET activation study with the Tower of London task. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[20]  J. Cummings,et al.  Frontal-subcortical circuits and human behavior. , 1993, Journal of psychosomatic research.

[21]  Carol A. Seger,et al.  Striatal activation during acquisition of a cognitive skill. , 1999, Neuropsychology.

[22]  D. Salmon,et al.  Neuropsychological evidence for multiple implicit memory systems: a comparison of Alzheimer's, Huntington's, and Parkinson's disease patients , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[23]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  Procedural learning is impaired in patients with prefrontal lesions , 1999, Neurology.

[24]  Daniel B. Willingham,et al.  Intact mirror-tracing and impaired rotary-pursuit skill learning in patients with Huntington's disease: evidence for dissociable memory systems in skill learning. , 1997, Neuropsychology.

[25]  E. Marder,et al.  Procedural memory in Parkinson's disease: impaired motor but not visuoperceptual learning. , 1990, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[26]  D. Perani,et al.  Evidence of multiple memory systems in the human brain. A [18F]FDG PET metabolic study. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[27]  C H Lücking,et al.  Changes in cortical activation during mirror reading before and after training: an fMRI study of procedural learning. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[28]  D. Salmon,et al.  Procedural learning in a patient with amnesia due to hypoxia , 1987, Brain and Cognition.

[29]  J. Desmond,et al.  The neural basis of visual skill learning: an fMRI study of mirror reading. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.

[30]  Axel Cleeremans,et al.  Striatum forever, despite sequence learning variability: A random effect analysis of PET data , 2000, Human brain mapping.

[31]  K Wienhard,et al.  Functional effects of striatal dysfunction in Parkinson disease. , 1997, Archives of neurology.

[32]  L. Cermak,et al.  Memory disorders associated with huntington's disease: Verbal recall, verbal recognition and procedural memory , 1985, Neuropsychologia.

[33]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. , 1986, Annual review of neuroscience.

[34]  John R. Anderson Acquisition of cognitive skill. , 1982 .

[35]  Niels Birbaumer,et al.  Memory and Skill Acquisition in Parkinson's Disease and Frontal Lobe Dysfunction , 1995, Cortex.