No evidence for impaired perception of biological motion in adults with autistic spectrum disorders

A central feature of autistic spectrum disorders (ASDs) is a difficulty in identifying and reading human expressions, including those present in the moving human form. One previous study, by Blake et al. (2003), reports decreased sensitivity for perceiving biological motion in children with autism, suggesting that perceptual anomalies underlie problems in social cognition. We revisited this issue using a novel psychophysical task. 16 adults with ASDs and 16 controls were asked to detect the direction of movement of human point-light walkers which were presented in both normal and spatially scrambled forms in a background of noise. Unlike convention direction discrimination tasks, in which walkers walk 'on the spot' while facing left or right, we added translatory motion to the stimulus so that the walkers physically moved across the screen. Therefore, while a cue of coherent, translatory motion was available in both the normal and scrambled walker forms, the normal walker alone contained information about the configuration and kinematics of the human body. There was a significant effect of walker type, with reduced response times and error when the normal walker was present. Most importantly, these improvements were the same for both participant groups, suggesting that people with ASDs do not have difficulty integrating local visual information into a global percept of the moving human form. The discrepancy between these and previous findings of impaired biological motion perception in ASDs are discussed with reference to differences in the age and diagnosis of the participants, and the nature of the task.

[1]  J. Faubert,et al.  Enhanced and diminished visuo-spatial information processing in autism depends on stimulus complexity. , 2005, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[2]  H. Engeland,et al.  Visual information processing in high-functioning individuals with autism spectrum disorders and their parents. , 2007, Neuropsychology.

[3]  S. Powis The Complete Guide to Asperger's Syndrome , 2006 .

[4]  M. Just,et al.  Sentence comprehension in autism: thinking in pictures with decreased functional connectivity. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[5]  Nikolaus F. Troje,et al.  Perception of biological motion in autism spectrum disorders , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind” ? , 1985, Cognition.

[7]  T. Allison,et al.  Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[8]  R. Adolphs,et al.  Cortical Regions for Judgments of Emotions and Personality Traits from Point-light Walkers , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[9]  S Baron-Cohen,et al.  Superior visual search in autism. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  S. Tsermentseli,et al.  Comparison of Form and Motion Coherence Processing in Autistic Spectrum Disorders and Dyslexia , 2008, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[11]  Arthur W Toga,et al.  Cortical sulcal maps in autism. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[12]  A. Cowey,et al.  Blindness to form from motion despite intact static form perception and motion detection , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  Daniel P. Kennedy,et al.  Functional abnormalities of the default network during self- and other-reflection in autism. , 2008, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[14]  Aina Puce,et al.  Electrophysiology and brain imaging of biological motion. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[15]  F. Volkmar,et al.  Validity and neuropsychological characterization of Asperger syndrome: convergence with nonverbal learning disabilities syndrome. , 1995, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[16]  Warren Jones,et al.  Altered face scanning and impaired recognition of biological motion in a 15-month-old infant with autism. , 2008, Developmental science.

[17]  Daniel Jokisch,et al.  Self recognition versus recognition of others by biological motion: viewpoint-dependent effects. , 2004, Perception.

[18]  J L Bradshaw,et al.  A Deficit in Shifting Attention Present in High-Functioning Autism but not Asperger’s Disorder , 2001, Autism : the international journal of research and practice.

[19]  Victor A. F. Lamme,et al.  Coherent versus Component Motion Perception in Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2008, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[20]  R. Blake,et al.  Brain Areas Involved in Perception of Biological Motion , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[21]  N. Troje,et al.  Person identification from biological motion: Effects of structural and kinematic cues , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.

[22]  Christoph Schmitz,et al.  The Developmental Neurobiology of Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Repetitive TMS over posterior STS disrupts perception of biological motion , 2005, Vision Research.

[24]  R. Igliozzi,et al.  Spatial and motion integration in children with autism , 2006, Vision Research.

[25]  G. Fink,et al.  Minds at rest? Social cognition as the default mode of cognizing and its putative relationship to the “default system” of the brain , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.

[26]  J A Beintema,et al.  Perception of biological motion without local image motion , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[27]  M. Sereno,et al.  Point-Light Biological Motion Perception Activates Human Premotor Cortex , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  S. Calhoun,et al.  Does DSM-IV Asperger's Disorder Exist? , 2001, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[29]  M. Pavlova,et al.  Orientation specificity in biological motion perception , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[30]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Neural basis of eye gaze processing deficits in autism. , 2005, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[31]  Janine V. Spencer,et al.  Visual Form-Processing Deficits in Autism , 2006, Perception.

[32]  Patricia Howlin,et al.  Outcome in High-Functioning Adults with Autism with and Without Early Language Delays: Implications for the Differentiation Between Autism and Asperger Syndrome , 2003, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[33]  Ruth A. Carper,et al.  Autism and Abnormal Development of Brain Connectivity , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  S. Calhoun,et al.  Ability Profiles in Children with Autism , 2003, Autism : the international journal of research and practice.

[35]  T. Allison,et al.  Temporal Cortex Activation in Humans Viewing Eye and Mouth Movements , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  Randolph Blake,et al.  Visual Recognition of Biological Motion is Impaired in Children With Autism , 2003, Psychological science.

[37]  G. Mather,et al.  Gender discrimination in biological motion displays based on dynamic cues , 1994, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[38]  R. Blake,et al.  Brain activity evoked by inverted and imagined biological motion , 2001, Vision Research.

[39]  M. Just,et al.  Functional connectivity in a baseline resting-state network in autism , 2006, Neuroreport.

[40]  Aina Puce,et al.  Configural Processing of Biological Motion in Human Superior Temporal Sulcus , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  Uta Frith,et al.  Theory of mind , 2001, Current Biology.

[42]  Lucia M Vaina,et al.  Perceptual deficits in patients with impaired recognition of biological motion after temporal lobe lesions. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[43]  S. Goldstein,et al.  The Comorbidity of Pervasive Developmental Disorder and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: Results of a Retrospective Chart Review , 2004, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[44]  Pierre Fonlupt,et al.  Listening to a walking human activates the temporal biological motion area , 2005, NeuroImage.

[45]  F A Wichmann,et al.  Ning for Helpful Comments and Suggestions. This Paper Benefited Con- Siderably from Conscientious Peer Review, and We Thank Our Reviewers the Psychometric Function: I. Fitting, Sampling, and Goodness of Fit , 2001 .

[46]  T. Poggio,et al.  Cognitive neuroscience: Neural mechanisms for the recognition of biological movements , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[47]  A. Couteur,et al.  Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders , 1994, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[48]  M. Brosnan,et al.  Gestalt processing in autism: failure to process perceptual relationships and the implications for contextual understanding. , 2004, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[49]  G. Johansson Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .

[50]  A. Saygin Superior temporal and premotor brain areas necessary for biological motion perception. , 2007, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[51]  J L Bradshaw,et al.  Atypical interference of local detail on global processing in high-functioning autism and Asperger's disorder. , 2000, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[52]  J. Atkinson,et al.  Normal and anomalous development of visual motion processing: motion coherence and ‘dorsal-stream vulnerability’ , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[53]  R. Blake,et al.  Brain Areas Active during Visual Perception of Biological Motion , 2002, Neuron.

[54]  F. Happé Autism: cognitive deficit or cognitive style? , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[55]  E. Bullmore,et al.  The amygdala theory of autism , 2000, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[56]  Neil A. Macmillan,et al.  Detection Theory: A User's Guide , 1991 .

[57]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Human Body , 2001 .

[58]  G. Dawson,et al.  Resting State Cortical Connectivity Reflected in EEG Coherence in Individuals With Autism , 2007, Biological Psychiatry.

[59]  Yoshiro Okubo,et al.  Enhanced activation in the extrastriate body area by goal‐directed actions , 2008, Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences.

[60]  Peter Newcombe,et al.  Theory of mind and central coherence in adults with high-functioning autism or Asperger syndrome , 2006, Autism : the international journal of research and practice.

[61]  Edward T. Bullmore,et al.  A direct demonstration of functional specialization within motion-related visual and auditory cortex of the human brain , 1996, Current Biology.

[62]  E. Pellicano,et al.  Investigating the functional integrity of the dorsal visual pathway in autism and dyslexia , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[63]  U. Frith,et al.  Vagaries of Visual Perception in Autism , 2005, Neuron.

[64]  Rp Hobson,et al.  Components of person perception: An investigation with autistic, non‐autistic retarded and typically developing children and adolescents , 1997 .

[65]  U. Frith,et al.  The Weak Coherence Account: Detail-focused Cognitive Style in Autism Spectrum Disorders , 2006, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[66]  Marcel Adam Just,et al.  Functional connectivity in an fMRI working memory task in high-functioning autism , 2005, NeuroImage.

[67]  Armin Bruderlin,et al.  Perceiving affect from arm movement , 2001, Cognition.

[68]  J E Cutting,et al.  Masking the motions of human gait , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[69]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Intention Understanding in Autism , 2009, PloS one.

[70]  U. Frith Emanuel Miller lecture: confusions and controversies about Asperger syndrome. , 2004, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[71]  D. Badcock,et al.  Abnormal global processing along the dorsal visual pathway in autism: a possible mechanism for weak visuospatial coherence? , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[72]  Bennett I. Bertenthal,et al.  Global Processing of Biological Motions , 1994 .

[73]  Geraldine Dawson,et al.  Abnormal functional connectivity in autism spectrum disorders during face processing. , 2008, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[74]  E. Courchesne,et al.  Face processing occurs outside the fusiform 'face area' in autism: evidence from functional MRI. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[75]  Bruce C Hansen,et al.  Peripheral vision: good for biological motion, bad for signal noise segregation? , 2007, Journal of vision.

[76]  M. Shiffrar,et al.  Recognizing people from their movement. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[77]  C. Mazefsky,et al.  The discriminative ability and diagnostic utility of the ADOS-G, ADI-R, and GARS for children in a clinical setting , 2006, Autism : the international journal of research and practice.

[78]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[79]  C. Frith,et al.  Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[80]  Richard S. Frackowiak,et al.  Superior temporal sulcus anatomical abnormalities in childhood autism: a voxel-based morphometry MRI study , 2004, NeuroImage.

[81]  E. Bullmore,et al.  The role of MT+/V5 during biological motion perception in Asperger Syndrome: An fMRI study , 2007 .

[82]  R. Campbell,et al.  High motion coherence thresholds in children with autism. , 2002, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[83]  J. Raven,et al.  Manual for Raven's progressive matrices and vocabulary scales , 1962 .

[84]  J. Haxby,et al.  Parallel Visual Motion Processing Streams for Manipulable Objects and Human Movements , 2002, Neuron.

[85]  N. Troje,et al.  The Inversion Effect in Biological Motion Perception: Evidence for a “Life Detector”? , 2006, Current Biology.

[86]  Andreia Santos,et al.  Recognition of biological motion in children with autistic spectrum disorders , 2008, Autism : the international journal of research and practice.

[87]  J. Faubert,et al.  Motion Perception in Autism: A Complex Issue , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[88]  Joseph P. McCleery,et al.  Abnormal Magnocellular Pathway Visual Processing in Infants at Risk for Autism , 2007, Biological Psychiatry.

[89]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Recognition of Mental State Terms , 1994, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[90]  B. Wicker,et al.  Brief Report: Recognition of Emotional and Non-emotional Biological Motion in Individuals with Autistic Spectrum Disorders , 2007, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[91]  Patrick Cavanagh,et al.  Perception of biological motion in parietal patients , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[92]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Perception of dynamic changes in facial affect and identity in autism. , 2007, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[93]  C. Dissanayake,et al.  Annotation: The similarities and differences between autistic disorder and Asperger's disorder: a review of the empirical evidence. , 2004, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[94]  F. Happé Studying weak central coherence at low levels: children with autism do not succumb to visual illusions. A research note. , 1996, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[95]  G. Orban,et al.  Specificity of regions processing biological motion , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[96]  Kevin J. Riggs,et al.  Motion processing in autism: evidence for a dorsal stream deficiency , 2000, Neuroreport.

[97]  D. Amaral,et al.  Neuroanatomy of autism , 2008, Trends in Neurosciences.

[98]  G. Pourtois,et al.  Distributed and interactive brain mechanisms during emotion face perception: Evidence from functional neuroimaging , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[99]  K. McKenzie The Complete Guide to Asperger's Syndrome , 2007 .

[100]  David J. Field,et al.  Contour integration by the human visual system: Evidence for a local “association field” , 1993, Vision Research.