Differences in memory dynamics between two closely related parasitoid wasp species

The two closely related parasitoids Cotesia glomerata and C. rubecula (Hymenoptera: Braconidae) coexist in The Netherlands where they occupy slightly different niches. When searching for their caterpillar hosts, they use host plant odours that are released upon feeding by the caterpillars. The species differ in their preference for plant odours during host searching after an associative learning experience. Cotesia glomerata changes its preference for the odour of a particular plant species after an oviposition experience on that plant, whereas C. rubecula does not alter its naive preference. Using no-choice wind tunnel bioassays we tested, for both species, to what extent oviposition induces memory formation and whether this results from associative learning. In experiment 1 we characterized the temporal dynamics of the memory trace. In both species, oviposition experience induced increased response levels compared to those of naive wasps. Memory dynamics differed between the species. A single associative learning experience induced a stable long-lasting memory trace that persisted for at least 5 days in C. glomerata. In C. rubecula a memory trace for the odour was present during the first day after the oviposition experience but waned over the following days. From a second experiment we concluded that the increased response could be attributed to a combination of nonassociative and associative learning. We furthermore formulate the learning paradigm for the parasitoids and hypothesize that adaptation to different spatial distributions of the preferred host species has led to the observed differences in memory dynamics.

[1]  J. V. van Loon,et al.  GC-EAG-analysis of volatiles from Brussels sprouts plants damaged by two species of Pieris caterpillars: olfactory receptive range of a specialist and a generalist parasitoid wasp species , 2002, CHEMOECOLOGY.

[2]  L. Vet,et al.  An evolutionary approach to host finding and selection. , 1986 .

[3]  A. Guo,et al.  Multiple-Phase Model of Memory Consolidation Confirmed by Behavioral and Pharmacological Analyses of Operant Conditioning in Drosophila , 1998, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[4]  L. Kaiser,et al.  Dynamics of odour learning in Leptopilina boulardi, a hymenopterous parasitoid , 2003, Animal Behaviour.

[5]  J. V. van Loon,et al.  Antennal sensilla of two parasitoid wasps: A comparative scanning electron microscopy study , 2004, Microscopy research and technique.

[6]  M. Dicke,et al.  Volatiles from damaged plants as major cues in long‐range host‐searching by the specialist parasitoid Cotesia rubecula , 1994 .

[7]  J. Takabayashi,et al.  Oviposition experience on a host‐infested plant affects flight and antennal searching behavior of Cotesia kariyai toward the host‐plant complex , 2000 .

[8]  W. J. Lewis,et al.  Host detection by chemically mediated associative learning in a parasitic wasp , 1988, Nature.

[9]  R. Menzel Searching for the memory trace in a mini-brain, the honeybee. , 2001, Learning & memory.

[10]  Louise E. M. Vet,et al.  Learning to discriminate between infochemicals from different plant‐host complexes by the parasitoids Cotesia glomerata and C. rubecula , 1998 .

[11]  J. Wiskerke,et al.  Foraging for solitarily and gregariously feeding caterpillars: A comparison of two related parasitoid species (Hymenoptera: Braconidae) , 1994, Journal of Insect Behavior.

[12]  L. Kaiser,et al.  In‐flight orientation to volatiles from the plant‐host complex in Cotesia rubecula (Hym.: Braconidae): increased sensitivity through olfactory experience , 1992 .

[13]  Lia Hemerik,et al.  Patch exploitation by the parasitoids Cotesia rubecula and Cotesia glomerata in multi‐patch environments with different host distributions , 1998 .

[14]  Tim Tully,et al.  Dissection of memory formation: from behavioral pharmacology to molecular genetics , 1995, Trends in Neurosciences.

[15]  R. Chapman,et al.  Perspectives in Chemoreception and Behavior , 1987, Proceedings in Life Sciences.

[16]  L. Vet,et al.  Comparison of learning in related generalist and specialist eucoilid parasitoids , 1992 .

[17]  R. Menzel,et al.  Cognitive architecture of a mini-brain: the honeybee , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[18]  M. Dicke,et al.  Relative importance of infochemicals from first and second trophic level in long-range host location by the larval parasitoidCotesia glomerata , 2004, Journal of Chemical Ecology.

[19]  M. Dicke,et al.  Response of the braconid parasitoid Cotesia (=Apanteles) glomerata to volatile infochemicals: effects of bioassay set‐up, parasitoid age and experience and barometric flux , 1992 .

[20]  W. J. Lewis,et al.  Learning of host searching cues by the larval parasitoid Microplitis croceipes , 2003 .

[21]  William J. Bell,et al.  Chemical Ecology of Insects , 1985, Springer US.

[22]  H. McAuslane,et al.  Stimuli influencing host microhabitat location in the parasitoid Campoletis sonorensis , 1991 .

[23]  Ann-Shyn Chiang,et al.  Neural substrates of memory: from synapse to system. , 2003, Journal of neurobiology.

[24]  R. Potting,et al.  Absence of odour learning in the stemborer parasitoid Cotesia flavipes , 1997, Animal Behaviour.

[25]  L. Kaiser,et al.  Odor learning byLeptopilina boulardi, a specialist parasitoid (Hymenoptera: Eucoilidae) , 1991, Journal of Insect Behavior.

[26]  W. J. Lewis,et al.  Parasitoid foraging and learning. , 1995 .

[27]  M. Dicke,et al.  Coexistence and niche segregation by field populations of the parasitoids Cotesia glomerata and C. rubecula in the Netherlands: predicting field performance from laboratory data , 2000, Oecologia.

[28]  R. Menzel Associative learning in honey bees , 1993 .

[29]  R. Menzel Memory dynamics in the honeybee , 1999, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[30]  J. Whitfield,et al.  Preliminary evolutionary relationships within the parasitoid wasp genus Cotesia (Hymenoptera: Braconidae: Microgastrinae): combined analysis of four genes , 2004 .

[31]  P. McCullagh,et al.  Generalized Linear Models , 1992 .

[32]  L. Vet,et al.  Effects of Pieris host species on life history parameters in a solitary specialist and gregarious generalist parasitoid (Cotesia species) , 1998 .

[33]  M. Dicke,et al.  Ecology of infochemical use by natural enemies in a tritrophic context. , 1992 .

[34]  Daniel R. Papaj,et al.  Insect learning: ecological and evolutionary perspectives. , 1992 .

[35]  A. Guo,et al.  Temporary Amnesia Induced by Cold Anesthesia and Hypoxia in Drosophila , 1998, Physiology & Behavior.

[36]  M. Dicke,et al.  Innate responses of the parasitoidsCotesia glomerata andC. rubecula (Hymenoptera: Braconidae) to volatiles from different plant-herbivore complexes , 1996, Journal of Insect Behavior.

[37]  Peter Kareiva,et al.  THE SEARCH FOR RESOURCES BY CABBAGE BUTTERFLIES (PIERIS RAPAE): ECOLOGICAL CONSEQUENCES AND ADAPTIVE SIGNIFICANCE OF MARKOVIAN MOVEMENTS IN A PATCHY ENVIRONMENT' , 1984 .

[38]  G. Edwards‐Jones,et al.  A review of the pre‐oviposition behaviour of small cabbage white butterfly, Pieris rapae(lepidoptera: Pieridae) , 1996 .

[39]  W. Lewis,et al.  Learning of Host-Finding Cues by Hymenopterous Parasitoids , 1993 .

[40]  J. Takabayashi,et al.  Learning of host‐infested plant volatiles in the larval parasitoid Cotesia kariyai , 2001 .

[41]  J. V. van Loon,et al.  Three-dimensional organization of the glomeruli in the antennal lobe of the parasitoid wasps Cotesia glomerata and C. rubecula , 2003, Cell and Tissue Research.

[42]  J. Harvey Dynamic effects of parasitism by an endoparasitoid wasp on the development of two host species: implications for host quality and parasitoid fitness , 2000 .

[43]  D. Laloi,et al.  Olfactory information transfer in the honeybee: compared efficiency of classical conditioning and early exposure , 2000, Animal Behaviour.

[44]  P. McCullagh,et al.  Generalized Linear Models , 1984 .

[45]  M. Dicke,et al.  Long-distance assessment of patch profitability through volatile infochemicals by the parasitoids Cotesia glomerata and C. rubecula (Hymenoptera: Braconidae). , 1998 .

[46]  A. L. Masurier,et al.  Costs and Benefits of Egg Clustering in Pieris brassicae , 1994 .