The effects of cognitive biases and imperfectness in long-term robot-human interactions: Case studies using five cognitive biases on three robots

The research presented in this paper demonstrates a model for aiding human-robot companionship based on the principle of human cognitive biases applied to a robot. The aim of this work was to study how cognitive biases can affect human-robot companionship in long-time. In the current paper, we show comparative results of the experiments using five biased algorithms in three different robots such as ERWIN, MyKeepon and MARC. The results were analysed to determine what difference if any of biased vs unbiased interaction has on the interaction with the robot and if the participants were able to form any kind of preference towards the different algorithms. The experimental presented show that the participants have more of a preference towards the biased algorithm interactions than the robot without the bias.

[1]  W. K. Campbell,et al.  Self-Threat Magnifies the Self-Serving Bias: A Meta-Analytic Integration , 1999 .

[2]  Brian R. Duffy,et al.  Anthropomorphism and the social robot , 2003, Robotics Auton. Syst..

[3]  J. Gross,et al.  PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES Emotion Regulation and Memory: The Cognitive Costs of Keeping One's Cool , 2004 .

[4]  R. Marois,et al.  Capacity limits of information processing in the brain , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[5]  A. G. Brand,et al.  Hot Cognition: Emotions and Writing Behavior. , 1986 .

[6]  R. McCrae,et al.  An introduction to the five-factor model and its applications. , 1992, Journal of personality.

[7]  K. M. Lee,et al.  Can robots manifest personality? : An empirical test of personality recognition, social responses, and social presence in human-robot interaction , 2006 .

[8]  J. Shepperd,et al.  Exploring Causes of the Self‐serving Bias , 2008 .

[9]  J. van der Pligt,et al.  Visceral Drives in Retrospect , 2006, Psychological science.

[10]  I. René J. A. te Boekhorst,et al.  Avoiding the uncanny valley: robot appearance, personality and consistency of behavior in an attention-seeking home scenario for a robot companion , 2008, Auton. Robots.

[11]  Elizabeth A. Crane,et al.  Methodology for Assessing Bodily Expression of Emotion , 2010 .

[12]  Brian Scassellati,et al.  Socially Assistive Robotics: Methods and Implications for the Future of Work and Care , 2022, Robophilosophy.

[13]  L. D. de Sonneville,et al.  Hot and Cool Forms of Inhibitory Control and Externalizing Behavior in Children of Mothers who Smoked during Pregnancy: An Exploratory Study , 2007, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[14]  A. Tversky,et al.  Subjective Probability: A Judgment of Representativeness , 1972 .

[15]  D. Buhrmester,et al.  Children's perceptions of the personal relationships in their social networks. , 1985 .

[16]  Piero Cosi,et al.  Long-term human-robot interaction with young users , 2011, HRI 2011.

[17]  Elizabeth C. Hirschman,et al.  Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases , 1974, Science.

[18]  Mriganka Biswas,et al.  Effect of cognitive biases on human-robot interaction: A case study of a robot's misattribution , 2014, The 23rd IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication.

[19]  Bilge Mutlu,et al.  Perceptions of ASIMO: an exploration on co-operation and competition with humans and humanoid robots , 2006, HRI '06.

[20]  B. Gansneder,et al.  Success and failure attributions of wrestlers: Further evidence of the self-serving bias. , 1998 .

[21]  Cynthia Breazeal,et al.  Social interactions in HRI: the robot view , 2004, IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics, Part C (Applications and Reviews).

[22]  Gary F. Russell,et al.  Interactive Perceptual Psychology: The Human Psychology That Mirrors the Naturalness of Human Behavior. , 1994 .

[23]  K. Fiedler,et al.  Social Cognition: How Individuals Construct Social Reality , 2004 .

[24]  Loran F. Nordgren,et al.  The Restraint Bias: How the Illusion of Self-Restraint Promotes Impulsive Behavior , 2009, Psychological science.

[25]  Takayuki Kanda,et al.  Analysis of Humanoid Appearances in Human–Robot Interaction , 2005, IEEE Transactions on Robotics.

[26]  G. L. White,et al.  Passionate love and the misattribution of arousal. , 1981 .

[27]  A. van Knippenberg,et al.  Finding comfort in a joke: consolatory effects of humor through cognitive distraction. , 2009, Emotion.

[28]  M. Haselton,et al.  The Evolution of Cognitive Bias , 2015 .

[29]  Myung Jin Chung,et al.  Artificial Emotion Generation Based on Personality, Mood, and Emotion for Life-Like Facial Expressions of Robots , 2010, HCIS.

[30]  Clifford Nass,et al.  Perceptual user interfaces: perceptual bandwidth , 2000, CACM.

[31]  K. Scherer,et al.  Cues and channels in emotion recognition. , 1986 .

[32]  G. Loewenstein,et al.  Exploring the Cold-to-Hot Empathy Gap in Smokers , 2008, Psychological science.

[33]  Robert Graham,et al.  Towards a tool for the Subjective Assessment of Speech System Interfaces (SASSI) , 2000, Natural Language Engineering.

[34]  Marc Hassenzahl,et al.  The Interplay of Beauty, Goodness, and Usability in Interactive Products , 2004, Hum. Comput. Interact..

[35]  Ronald C. Arkin,et al.  Time-Varying Affective Response for Humanoid Robots , 2009, FIRA.

[36]  G. Macdonald,et al.  Empathy gaps for social pain: why people underestimate the pain of social suffering. , 2011, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[37]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Retrieval without Recollection: An Experimental Analysis of Source Amnesia , 1984 .

[38]  Christoph Bartneck,et al.  Iterative design process for robots with personality , 2009 .

[39]  George Loewenstein,et al.  The Heat of the Moment: The Effect of Sexual Arousal on Sexual Decision Making , 2006 .

[40]  David G. Myers,et al.  Exploring Social Psychology , 1993 .

[41]  M. Hilbert,et al.  Toward a synthesis of cognitive biases: how noisy information processing can bias human decision making. , 2012, Psychological bulletin.

[42]  H. Simon,et al.  A Behavioral Model of Rational Choice , 1955 .

[43]  G Gigerenzer,et al.  Reasoning the fast and frugal way: models of bounded rationality. , 1996, Psychological review.

[44]  J. Kruger,et al.  Unskilled and unaware of it: how difficulties in recognizing one's own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[45]  W K Campbell,et al.  Among friends? An examination of friendship and the self-serving bias. , 2000, The British journal of social psychology.

[46]  Kerstin Dautenhahn,et al.  Would You Trust a (Faulty) Robot? Effects of Error, Task Type and Personality on Human-Robot Cooperation and Trust , 2015, 2015 10th ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI).

[47]  RobinsBen,et al.  KASPAR --a minimally expressive humanoid robot for human--robot interaction research , 2009 .

[48]  Tony Belpaeme,et al.  What a robotic companion could do for a diabetic child , 2014, The 23rd IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication.

[49]  H. Pfister,et al.  The multiplicity of emotions: A framework of emotional functions in decision making , 2008, Judgment and Decision Making.

[50]  D. Schacter,et al.  Misattribution, false recognition and the sins of memory. , 2001, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[51]  G. Mandler Origins of the cognitive (r)evolution. , 2002, Journal of the history of the behavioral sciences.