Reverberant auditory environments: the effects of multiple echoes on distraction by ‘irrelevant’ speech

SUMMARY Two experiments examine the effect on an immediate recall test of simulating a reverberant auditory environment in which auditory distracters in the form of speech are played to the participants (the ‘irrelevant sound effect’). An echo-intensive environment simulated by the addition of reverberation to the speech reduced the extent of ‘changes in state’ in the irrelevant speech stream by smoothing the profile of the waveform. In both experiments, the reverberant auditory environment produced significantly smaller irrelevant sound distraction effects than an echo-free environment. Results are interpreted in terms of changing-state hypothesis, which states that acoustic content of irrelevant sound, rather than phonology or semantics, determines the extent of the irrelevant sound effect (ISE). Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

[1]  Roy A. Ruddle,et al.  Spatial knowledge and virtual environments , 1999 .

[2]  C Philip Beaman,et al.  The irrelevant sound phenomenon revisited: what role for working memory capacity? , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[3]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Irrelevant tones produce an irrelevant speech effect : Implications for phonological coding in working memory , 1993 .

[4]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  The intrusiveness of sound: Laboratory findings and their implications for noise abatement. , 2001, Noise & health.

[5]  Philip Beaman Irrelevant sound effects amongst younger and older adults: Objective findings and subjective insights , 2005 .

[6]  Nick Mosdell,et al.  The role of habituation in the disruption of recall performance by irrelevant sound , 1997 .

[7]  Aimée M. Surprenant,et al.  Irrelevant Speech, Phonological Similarity, and Presentation Modality , 1999 .

[8]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Disruption of visual short-term memory by changing-state auditory stimuli: The role of segmentation , 1993, Memory & cognition.

[9]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Privileged Access by Irrelevant Speech to Short-term Memory: The Role of Changing State , 1992, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[10]  D. Berry,et al.  Disruption of office‐related tasks by speech and office noise , 1998 .

[11]  W. Ellermeier,et al.  Individual differences in susceptibility to the "irrelevant speech effect". , 1997, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[12]  J V Baranski,et al.  The effects of background noise on cognitive performance during a 70 hour simulation of conditions aboard the International Space Station. , 2003, Noise & health.

[13]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Irrelevant sound disrupts order information in free recall as in serial recall. , 1998 .

[14]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Objects, streams, and threads of auditory attention. , 1993 .

[15]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Word Dose in the Disruption of Serial Recall by Irrelevant Speech: Phonological Confusions or Changing State? , 1996 .

[16]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Auditory babble and cognitive efficiency: Role of number of voices and their location , 1995 .

[17]  I. Neath Modeling the effects of irrelevant speech on memory , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[18]  Alastair P. Nicholls,et al.  The irrelevant sound effect: does speech play a special role? , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[19]  Roy A. Ruddle,et al.  Navigating Large-Scale Desk-Top Virtual Buildings: Effects of Orientation Aids and Familiarity , 1998, Presence.

[20]  Dianne C. Berry,et al.  Habituation and dishabituation to speech and office noise , 1997 .

[21]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Disruption of proofreading by irrelevant speech: Effects of attention, arousal or memory? , 1990 .

[22]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Role of Habituation in the Irrelevant Sound Effect: Evidence From the Effects of Token Set Size and Rate of Transition , 1998 .

[23]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Organizational factors in selective attention: The interplay of acoustic distinctiveness and auditory streaming in the irrelevant sound effect , 1999 .

[24]  C. Philip Beaman,et al.  Auditory distraction from low‐intensity noise: a review of the consequences for learning and workplace environments , 2005 .

[25]  John P. Wann,et al.  Chapter 13 The use of virtual environments in perception action research: Grasping the impossible and controlling the improbable , 1995 .

[26]  Simon K. Rushton,et al.  Rehabilitative environments for attention and movement disorders , 1997, CACM.

[27]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Role of Serial Order in the Irrelevant Speech Effect: Tests of the Changing-State Hypothesis , 1997 .

[28]  N. Cowan Attention and Memory: An Integrated Framework , 1995 .

[29]  C. P. Beaman,et al.  The object-oriented episodic record model , 1996 .

[30]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Interference from degraded auditory stimuli: linear effects of changing-state in the irrelevant sequence. , 2000, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[31]  Alan D. Baddeley,et al.  Disruption of short-term memory by unattended speech : Implications for the structure of working memory , 1982 .

[32]  J. S. Nairne A feature model of immediate memory , 1990, Memory & cognition.

[33]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Indispensable benefits and unavoidable costs of unattended sound for cognitive functioning. , 2003, Noise & health.

[34]  D. C. Lecompte,et al.  The importance of semantic similarity to the irrelevant speech effect , 1999, Memory & cognition.

[35]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Auditory Distraction and Short-Term Memory: Phenomena and Practical Implications , 2001, Hum. Factors.

[36]  A. Watson,et al.  Architecture and sound: an acoustic analysis of megalithic monuments in prehistoric Britain , 1999, Antiquity.

[37]  Nelson Cowan,et al.  Coherence of the irrelevant-sound effect: Individual profiles of short-term memory and susceptibility to task-irrelevant materials , 2005, Memory & cognition.

[38]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Disruption by speech of serial short-term memory: The role of changing-state vowels , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[39]  D C LeCompte,et al.  Extending the irrelevant speech effect beyond serial recall. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[40]  D C LeCompte,et al.  Irrelevant speech and irrelevant tones: the relative importance of speech to the irrelevant speech effect. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[41]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  The phonological store of working memory: is it phonological and is it a store? , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[42]  Bill Macken,et al.  Role of Habituation in the Irrelevant Sound Effect : Evidence From the Effects of Token Set Size and Rate of Transition , 2001 .

[43]  M. Forte,et al.  Virtual Reality in archaeology , 2000 .

[44]  Alan Baddeley,et al.  Disruption of Verbal Stm by Irrelevant Speech, Articulatory Suppression, and Manual Tapping: Do they have a Common Source? , 2003, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.