Investigating the Syntax Line: Configurational Properties and Cognitive Correlates

Space syntax research has had significant success over the years and has served to illustrate the importance of configurational measures, especially those that take into account all the spaces in a system. Here, assumptions of axial lines as elementary spatial units have been overwhelming. Although based on a theoretical construct of visibility, this postulation has rarely been extended to perception or cognition and this has given rise to questions about geometric and metric considerations. The research presented here was carried out in three large urban hospitals. In them, 128 volunteers performed ‘open searches’, where they attempted to become familiar with the hospital; ‘directed searches’, where they sought specific locations; and various cognitive mapping tasks such as pointing to locations that were not within sight and sketching the main corridors and routes of the hospital. The hospitals themselves were analyzed through conventional syntax measures of axial lines and a segmented version of those lines. Correlational and regression analyses revealed that, although use of space was best predicted by local measures (connectivity), distribution of people was better explained through integration-3. Performance comparisons between the environmental measures given by ‘whole’ and ‘segmented’ lines suggested that, at least in complex architectural settings, original syntax definitions of axial lines as uninterrupted visibility lines have more predictive power and better cognitive presence than ‘segmented’ lines. The results also support previous findings of intelligibility as a factor in predicting space use by extending it to the cognitive realm. Additionally, it has brought out the reverse role of intelligibility-3. It was also found that mean depth of an entry effects the way a building is explored; therefore, knowledge of this spatial property may provide a fair indication of the relative importance which each space will have on wayfinding.

[1]  Tommy Gärling,et al.  Spatial orientation and wayfinding in the designed environment: A conceptual analysis and some suggestions for postoccupancy evaluation. , 1986 .

[2]  Mark D. Gross,et al.  Predicting Wayfinding Behavior in Buildings: a Schema-Based Approach , 1992 .

[3]  Sara Girotto,et al.  Wayfinding and environmental cognition in the designed environment , 2003 .

[4]  Harry Heft,et al.  Way -finding as the perception of information over time , 1983 .

[5]  E. Lindberg,et al.  Cognitive Mapping of Large-Scale Environments , 1984 .

[6]  Bill Hillier,et al.  The Hidden Geometry of Deformed Grids: Or, Why Space Syntax Works, When it Looks as Though it Shouldn't , 1999 .

[7]  Bill Hillier,et al.  A theory of the city as object: or, how spatial laws mediate the social construction of urban space , 2001 .

[8]  Harry Heft The Relevance of Gibson’s Ecological Approach to Perception for Environment-Behavior Studies , 1997 .

[9]  Benjamin Kuipers,et al.  Modeling Spatial Knowledge , 1978, IJCAI.

[10]  A. Penn Space Syntax And Spatial Cognition , 2003 .

[11]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[12]  Y K Choi The Morphology of Exploration and Encounter in Museum Layouts , 1999 .

[13]  C. Zimring,et al.  Just Down The Road A Piece , 2003 .

[14]  L Hamayon,et al.  Direction - Finding in Large Buildings , 1969 .

[15]  A. Siegel,et al.  The development of spatial representations of large-scale environments. , 1975, Advances in child development and behavior.

[16]  Harry Heft,et al.  The Ecological Approach to Navigation: A Gibsonian Perspective , 1996 .

[17]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Navigator: A psychologically based model of environmental learning through navigation , 1989 .

[18]  J. Peponis,et al.  Finding the Building in Wayfinding , 1990 .

[19]  E. Reed The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1989 .

[20]  Jerry Weisman,et al.  Evaluating Architectural Legibility , 1981 .

[21]  T. Gärling,et al.  The effects of pathway configuration, landmarks and stress on environmental cognition , 1984 .

[22]  H Gradmann [Orientation in space]. , 1971, Studium generale; Zeitschrift fur die Einheit der Wissenschaften im Zusammenhang ihrer Begriffsbildungen und Forschungsmethoden.

[23]  S. Haq Complex architectural settings :an investigation of spatial and cognitive variables through wayfinding behavior , 2001 .

[24]  Bill Hillier,et al.  The architecture of the urban object , 1989 .

[25]  F. J. Langdon,et al.  The Child's Conception of Space , 1967 .

[26]  William J. Cook,et al.  Human Orientation in Transportation Terminals , 1980 .

[27]  Michael O'Neill,et al.  Evaluation of a Conceptual Model of Architectural Legibility , 1991 .

[28]  Bill Hillier,et al.  Specifically architectural theory: a partial account of the ascent from building as cultural transmission to architecture as theoretical concretion , 1993 .

[29]  Avishai Henik,et al.  A comparison of map study methods: simulated travel vs conventional study , 1987 .

[30]  Alan Penn,et al.  Creating Life: Or, Does Architecture Determine Anything? , 1987 .

[31]  Donald Appleyard,et al.  Why Buildings Are Known , 1969 .

[32]  G. Evans,et al.  Cognitive mapping and architecture. , 1980 .

[33]  Young Ook Kim The Role of Spatial Configuration in Spatial Cognition , 2001 .

[34]  Mahbub Rashid,et al.  On the Description of Shape and Spatial Configuration inside Buildings: Convex Partitions and Their Local Properties , 1997 .

[35]  Michael J. Rovine,et al.  Sketch-map variables as predictors of way-finding performance , 1989 .

[36]  Julienne Hanson,et al.  Syntactic Analysis of Settlements , 1987 .

[37]  R. Golledge,et al.  Behavioral problems in geography : a symposium , 1971 .