Gamma flicker triggers attentional selection without awareness

Gamma band modulations in neural activity have been proposed to mediate attentional processes. To support a causal link between gamma activity and attentional selection, we attempt to evoke gamma oscillations by a 50-Hz subliminal flicker. We find that a subliminal 50-Hz flicker at a target location, before target presentation, speeds up and enhances target detection and discrimination. This effect is specific to the middle of the gamma range because it is not evident at <35-Hz flicker. It requires 300 ms to build up, dissipates within 250 ms of flicker offset, and shows a tendency to invert after 500 ms. The results are discussed in relation to a role for gamma band neural synchrony in the allocation of visual attention.

[1]  C. Koch,et al.  Towards a neurobiological theory of consciousness , 1990 .

[2]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[3]  Pieter R. Roelfsema,et al.  Neuronal synchronization is a correlate of perceptual dominance in awake squinting cats , 1996 .

[4]  Ernst Niebur,et al.  High-frequency gamma activity (80–150Hz) is increased in human cortex during selective attention , 2008, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[5]  D. Tolhurst,et al.  Psychophysical evidence for sustained and transient detectors in human vision , 1973, The Journal of physiology.

[6]  Steven C Dakin,et al.  Role of synchrony in contour binding: some transient doubts sustained. , 2002, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[7]  W. Singer,et al.  Rapid feature selective neuronal synchronization through correlated latency shifting , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[8]  M M Sondhi,et al.  Model for visual luminance discrimination and flicker detection. , 1968, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[9]  C. Gray,et al.  Adaptive Coincidence Detection and Dynamic Gain Control in Visual Cortical Neurons In Vivo , 2003, Neuron.

[10]  Catherine Tallon-Baudry,et al.  Visual Grouping and the Focusing of Attention Induce Gamma-band Oscillations at Different Frequencies in Human Magnetoencephalogram Signals , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[11]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of spatial selective attention in areas V1, V2, and V4 of macaque visual cortex. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  S. Epstein,et al.  Background gamma rhythmicity and attention in cortical local circuits: a computational study. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[13]  T. Womelsdorf,et al.  The role of neuronal synchronization in selective attention , 2007, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[14]  W. Singer,et al.  Dynamic predictions: Oscillations and synchrony in top–down processing , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[15]  Denis Cousineau,et al.  Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson's method , 2005 .

[16]  J. Robson Spatial and Temporal Contrast-Sensitivity Functions of the Visual System , 1966 .

[17]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Saccades, salience and attention: the role of the lateral intraparietal area in visual behavior. , 2006, Progress in brain research.

[18]  R. Desimone,et al.  Selective attention gates visual processing in the extrastriate cortex. , 1985, Science.

[19]  C. Herrmann Human EEG responses to 1–100 Hz flicker: resonance phenomena in visual cortex and their potential correlation to cognitive phenomena , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  James Gordon,et al.  Entrainment to Video Displays in Primary Visual Cortex of Macaque and Humans , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  G. Laurent,et al.  Who reads temporal information contained across synchronized and oscillatory spike trains? , 1998, Nature.

[22]  Christof Koch,et al.  A model for the neuronal implementation of selective visual attention based on temporal correlation among neurons , 1994, Journal of Computational Neuroscience.

[23]  C. Tallon-Baudry,et al.  Neural Dissociation between Visual Awareness and Spatial Attention , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  Heidi S. Fisher,et al.  Adaptation from invisible flicker. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[25]  P. McCormick Orienting attention without awareness. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  W. Freiwald,et al.  Coherent oscillatory activity in monkey area v4 predicts successful allocation of attention. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[27]  E. Niebur,et al.  Growth patterns in the developing brain detected by using continuum mechanical tensor maps , 2022 .

[28]  R. Desimone,et al.  Modulation of Oscillatory Neuronal Synchronization by Selective Visual Attention , 2001, Science.

[29]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  Correlated neuronal activity and the flow of neural information , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[30]  M. Posner,et al.  Neural systems control of spatial orienting. , 1982, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[31]  R. Desimone,et al.  Gamma-band synchronization in visual cortex predicts speed of change detection , 2006, Nature.

[32]  W. Singer,et al.  Synchronization of oscillatory responses in visual cortex correlates with perception in interocular rivalry. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[33]  G W Humphreys,et al.  Luminance-increment detection: capacity-limited or not? , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[34]  Jorge V. José,et al.  Inhibitory synchrony as a mechanism for attentional gain modulation , 2004, Journal of Physiology-Paris.

[35]  Ping Wei,et al.  Searching for two feature singletons in the visual scene: the localized attentional interference effect , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.