Abnormal Semantic Network for “Animals” But not “Tools” in Patients with Alzheimer's Disease

The effects of Alzheimer's disease (AD) on the organization of semantic knowledge (i.e., the semantic network) for living (e.g., animals) and non-living (e.g., tools) categories was examined. Multidimensional scaling and Pathfinder analyses of data from triadic comparison tasks showed that the semantic network for "animals", but not the network for "tools", was abnormal in patients with AD. Specifically, patients with AD tended to use a different primary dimension than control subjects for categorizing animals and their network was characterized by atypical associations between concepts. The differences in the integrity of the AD patients' networks for "animals" and "tools" was not likely to be an artifact of differences in the difficulty in identifying the stimuli in the two categories as all stimuli were identified on simple naming or matching tasks. These findings support the results of previous studies that have shown the presence of category-specific semantic deficits in patients with AD.

[1]  Marie-Claire Goldblum,et al.  The influence of semantic and perceptual encoding on recognition memory in Alzheimer’s disease , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  N. Birbaumer,et al.  Semantic memory impairment in Alzheimer's disease. , 1996, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[3]  Murray Grossman,et al.  The Semantic Memory Impairment of Alzheimer's Disease: Category-Specific? , 1996, Cortex.

[4]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[5]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Calling a squirrel a squirrel but a canoe a wigwam: a category-specific deficit for artefactual objects and body parts , 1992 .

[6]  R. Nebes,et al.  Instantiation of semantic categories in sentence comprehension by Alzheimer patients , 1999, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[7]  E. Warrington,et al.  Evidence for modality-specific meaning systems in the brain , 1988, Nature.

[8]  Lance J. Rips,et al.  Semantic distance and the verification of semantic relations , 1973 .

[9]  F. Boller,et al.  Classification Deficits in Alzheimer's Disease with Special Reference to Living and Nonliving Things , 1996, Brain and Language.

[10]  G. Gainotti,et al.  Dissociation between knowledge of living and nonliving things in dementia of the Alzheimer type , 1991, Neurology.

[11]  F. Craik,et al.  Hemispheric encoding/retrieval asymmetry in episodic memory: positron emission tomography findings. , 1994, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[12]  Jane S. Paulsen,et al.  An Assessment of the Semantic Network in Patients with Alzheimer's Disease , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[13]  F. Boller,et al.  The naming impairment of living and nonliving items in Alzheimer's disease , 1995, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[14]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  A verbal-semantic category-specific recognition impairment , 1993 .

[15]  D. Salmon,et al.  Semantic memory impairment in Alzheimer's disease: Failure of access or degraded knowledge? , 1992, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  Allan Collins,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing , 1975 .

[17]  W. Montague,et al.  Category norms of verbal items in 56 categories A replication and extension of the Connecticut category norms , 1969 .

[18]  D. Bub,et al.  Semantic memory loss in dementia of Alzheimer's type. What do various measures measure? , 1990, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[19]  Y. Goto,et al.  Mitochondrial myopathy, encephalopathy, lactic acidosis, and stroke‐like episodes (MELAS) , 1992, Neurology.

[20]  R. Katzman.,et al.  Pathological verification of ischemic score in differentiation of dementias , 1980, Annals of neurology.

[21]  Robert D. Nebes The cognitive neuropsychology of Alzheimer's disease. , 2000 .

[22]  D. W. Dearholt,et al.  Properties of pathfinder networks , 1990 .

[23]  N Butters,et al.  Semantic network abnormality predicts rate of cognitive decline in patients with probable Alzheimer's disease , 1995, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[24]  M. Folstein,et al.  Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease , 1984, Neurology.

[25]  F. Lucchelli,et al.  Are Semantic Systems Separately Represented in the Brain? The Case of Living Category Impairment , 1994, Cortex.

[26]  Jane S. Paulsen,et al.  Comparison of the semantic networks in patients with dementia and amnesia. , 1995, Neuropsychology.

[27]  L. Squire,et al.  The Neuropsychology of Memory , 1990 .

[28]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge , 1996, Nature.

[29]  Elizabeth K. Warrington,et al.  Neuropsychological studies of verbal semantic systems , 1981 .

[30]  L. Bäckman,et al.  Influences of cognitive support on episodic remembering: tracing the process of loss from normal aging to Alzheimer's disease. , 1998, Psychology and aging.

[31]  S. Corkin,et al.  Category knowledge in Alzheimer's disease: normal organization and a general retrieval deficit. , 1992, Psychology and aging.

[32]  C. White,et al.  The role of cortical connectivity in Alzheimer's disease pathogenesis: A review and model system , 1993, Neurobiology of Aging.

[33]  D. Salmon,et al.  The deterioration of semantic networks in patients with Alzheimer's disease: A cross-sectional study , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  Marcia K. Johnson,et al.  Semantic space in Alzheimer's disease patients. , 1995 .

[35]  Carol McCleary The semantic organization and classification of fourteen words by aphasic patients , 1988, Brain and Language.

[36]  T. Shallice,et al.  Category specific semantic impairments , 1984 .

[37]  P Garrard,et al.  Category specific semantic loss in dementia of Alzheimer's type. Functional-anatomical correlations from cross-sectional analyses. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[38]  M. M. Meyer,et al.  Can recognotion of living things be selectively impaired? , 1991, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  J. Chang,et al.  Analysis of individual differences in multidimensional scaling via an n-way generalization of “Eckart-Young” decomposition , 1970 .

[40]  Anna Maria Di Betta,et al.  The Production of Specific and Generic Associates of Living and Nonliving, High- and Low-Familiarity Stimuli in Alzheimer's Disease , 1996, Brain and Language.

[41]  T. Goldsmith,et al.  Assessing structural similarity of graphs , 1990 .

[42]  N Birbaumer,et al.  Category-specific semantic impairment in Alzheimer's disease and temporal lobe dysfunction: a comparative study. , 1994, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[43]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Category-specific naming and comprehension impairment: a double dissociation. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[44]  M. Farah,et al.  A category-specific naming impairment after temporal lobectomy , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[45]  E. Warrington,et al.  CATEGORY SPECIFIC ACCESS DYSPHASIA , 1983 .

[46]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Double Dissociation of Semantic Categories in Alzheimer's Disease , 1997, Brain and Language.

[47]  D. Salmon,et al.  Differential decline in word generation from phonemic and semantic categories during the course of Alzheimer's disease: Implications for the integrity of semantic memory , 1999, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[48]  N Butters,et al.  Episodic and semantic memory: a comparison of amnesic and demented patients. , 1987, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[49]  Gregory Ashby,et al.  On the Dangers of Averaging Across Subjects When Using Multidimensional Scaling or the Similarity-Choice Model , 1994 .

[50]  Roger W. Schvaneveldt,et al.  Pathfinder associative networks: studies in knowledge organization , 1990 .

[51]  Giuseppe Sartori,et al.  The oyster with four legs: A neuropsychological study on the interaction of visual and semantic information , 1998 .

[52]  Roger N. Shepard,et al.  Multidimensional scaling : theory and applications in the behavioral sciences , 1974 .

[53]  K. Jellinger,et al.  Neuropathological staging of Alzheimer lesions and intellectual status in Alzheimer's and Parkinson's disease patients , 1993, Neuroscience Letters.

[54]  Richard D. Hichwa,et al.  A neural basis for lexical retrieval , 1996, Nature.

[55]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Discrete Cortical Regions Associated with Knowledge of Color and Knowledge of Action , 1995, Science.

[56]  M. Ross Quillian,et al.  Retrieval time from semantic memory , 1969 .

[57]  N. Henley A psychological study of the semantics of animal terms , 1969 .

[58]  Myrna F. Schwartz,et al.  Of cabbages and things: Semantic memory from a neuropsychological perspective—A tutorial review. , 1994 .

[59]  J Marshall,et al.  Cerebral blood flow in dementia. , 1975, Archives of neurology.

[60]  David P. Salmon,et al.  Dimensionality and clustering in the semantic network of patients with Alzheimer's disease. , 1993 .

[61]  G. Barba,et al.  The influence of semantic encoding on recognition memory in Alzheimer's disease , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[62]  Alex Martin,et al.  Word production and comprehension in Alzheimer's diseáse: The breakdown of semantic knowledge , 1983, Brain and Language.

[63]  M. Silveri,et al.  Interaction between vision and language in category-specific semantic impairment , 1988 .

[64]  E. Warrington,et al.  Categories of knowledge. Further fractionations and an attempted integration. , 1987, Brain : a journal of neurology.