Examining the Presence and Determinants of Operational Momentum in Childhood
暂无分享,去创建一个
Steffen Zitzmann | Koleen McCrink | André Knops | A. Knops | Steffen Zitzmann | Koleen McCrink | K. McCrink
[1] V. Michel,et al. Recruitment of an Area Involved in Eye Movements During Mental Arithmetic , 2009, Science.
[2] ROBERT S. MOYER,et al. Time required for Judgements of Numerical Inequality , 1967, Nature.
[3] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Dynamic representations underlying symbolic and nonsymbolic calculation: Evidence from the operational momentum effect , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[4] M. Corbetta,et al. The Reorienting System of the Human Brain: From Environment to Theory of Mind , 2008, Neuron.
[5] K. Wynn,et al. Large-Number Addition and Subtraction by 9-Month-Old Infants , 2004, Psychological science.
[6] Samuel Shaki,et al. Reading habits for both words and numbers contribute to the SNARC effect , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[7] P. Onghena,et al. The relationship between the shape of the mental number line and familiarity with numbers in 5- to 9-year old children: evidence for a segmented linear model. , 2008, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[8] Tom Verguts,et al. Spatial Intuition in Elementary Arithmetic: A Neurocomputational Account , 2012, PloS one.
[9] Yves Rossetti,et al. Interference between number processing and line bisection: a methodology , 2005, Neuropsychologia.
[10] Clarissa A. Thompson,et al. Early development of spatial-numeric associations: evidence from spatial and quantitative performance of preschoolers. , 2010, Developmental science.
[11] Melissa E. Libertus,et al. Comment on "Log or Linear? Distinct Intuitions of the Number Scale in Western and Amazonian Indigene Cultures" , 2009, Science.
[12] R Core Team,et al. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .
[13] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Development of Elementary Numerical Abilities: A Neuronal Model , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[14] 永福 智志. The Organization of Learning , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[15] M. Carrasco. Visual attention: The past 25 years , 2011, Vision Research.
[16] Robert S. Siegler,et al. Representational change and children’s numerical estimation , 2007, Cognitive Psychology.
[17] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Moving along the Number Line: Operational Momentum in Nonsymbolic Arithmetic , 2006 .
[18] M. H. Fischer,et al. Number processing induces spatial performance biases , 2001, Neurology.
[19] Nancy Kanwisher,et al. Non-symbolic arithmetic in adults and young children , 2006, Cognition.
[20] Simon Jackman,et al. Bayesian Analysis for the Social Sciences , 2009 .
[21] Michael D. Dodd,et al. Perceiving numbers causes spatial shifts of attention , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[22] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Calibrating the mental number line , 2008, Cognition.
[23] E. Miller,et al. Coding of Cognitive Magnitude Compressed Scaling of Numerical Information in the Primate Prefrontal Cortex , 2003, Neuron.
[24] R. Siegler,et al. The Development of Numerical Estimation , 2003, Psychological science.
[25] Koleen McCrink,et al. Operational momentum in large-number addition and subtraction by 9-month-olds. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[26] Pieter Reitsma,et al. Developing access to number magnitude: a study of the SNARC effect in 7- to 9-year-olds. , 2008, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[27] D. Berch,et al. Extracting parity and magnitude from Arabic numerals: developmental changes in number processing and mental representation. , 1999, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[28] H. Huynh,et al. Conditions under Which Mean Square Ratios in Repeated Measurements Designs Have Exact F-Distributions , 1970 .
[29] W. Fias,et al. The development of the SNARC effect: evidence for early verbal coding. , 2012, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[30] R. Wilcox. A Heteroscedastic ANOVA Procedure With Specified Power , 1987 .
[31] Marco Zorzi,et al. Numerical estimation in preschoolers. , 2010, Developmental psychology.
[32] Marco Zorzi,et al. Emergence of a 'visual number sense' in hierarchical generative models , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.
[33] W. Meck,et al. Common Representations of Abstract Quantities , 2007 .
[34] J. Kruschke. Doing Bayesian Data Analysis: A Tutorial with R and BUGS , 2010 .
[35] K. Priftis,et al. Brain damage: Neglect disrupts the mental number line , 2002, Nature.
[36] Hilary C Barth,et al. The development of numerical estimation: evidence against a representational shift. , 2011, Developmental science.
[37] S. Dehaene,et al. Representation of number in the brain. , 2009, Annual review of neuroscience.
[38] Marco Zorzi,et al. Representation of numerical and non-numerical order in children , 2012, Cognition.
[39] Stephan F. Taylor,et al. Removing the effect of response time on brain activity reveals developmental differences in conflict processing in the posterior medial prefrontal cortex , 2012, NeuroImage.
[40] C. Gallistel,et al. Non-verbal numerical cognition: from reals to integers , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[41] Peter Brugger,et al. Stimulus-response compatibility in representational space , 1998, Neuropsychologia.
[42] M. Corbetta,et al. Quantitative analysis of attention and detection signals during visual search. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.
[43] Martin H. Fischer,et al. Mental movements without magnitude? A study of spatial biases in symbolic arithmetic , 2008, Cognition.
[44] Kristina M. Visscher,et al. A Core System for the Implementation of Task Sets , 2006, Neuron.
[45] Wim Gevers,et al. Look, no hands: A perceptual task shows that number magnitude induces shifts of attention , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[46] E. Schröger,et al. The development of involuntary and voluntary attention from childhood to adulthood: A combined behavioral and event-related potential study , 2006, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[47] Jin Fan,et al. Development of attentional networks: An fMRI study with children and adults , 2005, NeuroImage.
[48] E. Schröger,et al. The cognitive control of distraction by novelty in children aged 7-8 and adults. , 2009, Psychophysiology.
[49] Brian Butterworth,et al. Mapping numerical magnitudes along the right lines: differentiating between scale and bias. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[50] Karen Wynn,et al. Addition and subtraction by human infants , 1992, Nature.
[51] Hilary Barth,et al. Abstract number and arithmetic in preschool children. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[52] S. Dehaene,et al. The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .
[53] Rand R. Wilcox,et al. New monte carlo results on the robustness of the anova f, w and f statistics , 1986 .
[54] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Number line compression and the illusory perception of random numbers. , 2010, Experimental psychology.
[55] C R Gallistel,et al. Numerical Subtraction in the Pigeon: Evidence for a Linear Subjective Number Scale , 2001, Psychological science.
[56] Maciej Haman,et al. The spatial-numerical congruity effect in preschoolers. , 2012, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[57] Samuel Shaki,et al. Direction counts: a comparative study of spatially directional counting biases in cultures with different reading directions. , 2012, Journal of experimental child psychology.