Head direction cell activity monitored in a novel environment and during a cue conflict situation.

1. Recent conceptualizations of the neural systems used during navigation have classified two types of sensory information used by animals: landmark cues and internally based (idiothetic; e.g., vestibular, kinesthetic) sensory cues. Previous studies have identified neurons in the postsubiculum and the anterior thalamic nuclei that discharge as a function of the animal's head direction in the horizontal plane. The present study was designed to determine how animals use head direction (HD) cells for spatial orientation and the types of sensory cues involved. 2. HD cell activity was monitored in the postsubiculum and anterior thalamic nucleus of rats in a dual-chamber apparatus in an experiment that consisted of two phases. In the first phase, HD cell activity was monitored as an animal moved from a familiar environment to a novel environment. It was hypothesized that if HD cells were capable of using idiothetic sensory information, then the direction of maximal discharge should remain relatively unchanged as the animal moved into an environment where it was unfamiliar with the landmark cues. In the second phase, HD cells were monitored under conditions in which a conflict situation was introduced between the established landmark cues and the animal's internally generated sensory cues. 3. HD cells were initially recorded in a cylinder containing a single orientation cue (familiar environment). A door was then opened, and the rat entered a U-shaped passageway leading to a rectangular chamber containing a different prominent cue (novel environment). For most HD cells, the preferred direction remained relatively constant between the cylinder and passageway/rectangle, although many cells showed a small (6-30 degrees) shift in their preferred direction in the novel environment. This directional shift was maintained across different episodes in the passageway/rectangle. 4. Before the next session, the orientation cue in the cylinder was rotated 90 degrees, and the animal returned to the cylinder. The cell's preferred direction usually shifted between 45 and 90 degrees in the same direction. 5. The rat was then permitted to walk back through the passageway into the now-familiar rectangle. Immediately upon entering the passageway, the preferred direction returned to its original (prerotation) orientation and remained at this value while the rat was in the rectangle. When the rat was allowed to walk back into the cylinder, one of three outcomes occurred: 1) the cell's preferred direction shifted, such that it remained linked to the cylinder's rotated cue card; 2) the cell's preferred direction remained unchanged from its orientation in the rectangle; or 3) the cell's preferred direction shifted to a new value that lay between the preferred directions for the rotated cylinder condition and rectangle. 6. There was little change in the HD cell's background firing rate, peak firing rate, or directional firing range for both the novel and cue-conflict situations. 7. Simultaneous recordings from multiple cells in different sessions showed that the preferred directions remained "in register" with one another. Thus, when one HD cell shifted its preferred direction a specific amount, the other HD cell also shifted its preferred direction the same amount. 8. Results across different series within the same animal showed that the amount the preferred direction shifted in the first Novel series was about the same amount as the shifts observed in subsequent Novel series. In contrast, as the animal experienced more Conflict series, HD cells tended to use the cylinder's cue card less as an orientation cue when the animal returned to the rotated cylinder condition from the rectangle. 9. These results suggest that HD cells in the postsubiculum and anterior thalamic nuclei receive information from both landmark and idiothetic sensory cues, and when both types of cues are available, HD cells preferentially use the landmark cues as long as they are perceived

[1]  J. O’Keefe Place units in the hippocampus of the freely moving rat , 1976, Experimental Neurology.

[2]  A. J. Hill First occurrence of hippocampal spatial firing in a new environment , 1978, Experimental Neurology.

[3]  R. Passingham The hippocampus as a cognitive map J. O'Keefe & L. Nadel, Oxford University Press, Oxford (1978). 570 pp., £25.00 , 1979, Neuroscience.

[4]  L. Abraham,et al.  Vestibular involvement in a passive transport and return task , 1983 .

[5]  G. Paxinos,et al.  The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates , 1983 .

[6]  J. Kubie A driveable bundle of microwires for collecting single-unit data from freely-moving rats , 1984, Physiology & Behavior.

[7]  M. Seki,et al.  Anterior thalamic afferents from the mamillary body and the limbic cortex in the rat , 1984, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[8]  J. B. Ranck,et al.  Spatial firing patterns of hippocampal complex-spike cells in a fixed environment , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[9]  Rotational stimulation disrupts spatial learning in fornix-lesioned rats. , 1988 .

[10]  S A Deadwyler,et al.  Rotational stimulation disrupts spatial learning in fornix-lesioned rats. , 1988, Behavioral neuroscience.

[11]  R. Sutherland,et al.  The role of the fornix/fimbria and some related subcortical structures in place learning and memory , 1989, Behavioural Brain Research.

[12]  D. Hopkins,et al.  Mamillary body in the rat: Topography and synaptology of projections from the subicular complex, prefrontal cortex, and midbrain tegmentum , 1989, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[13]  J Bures,et al.  Vestibular stimulation disrupts acquisition of place navigation in the Morris water tank task. , 1989, Behavioral and neural biology.

[14]  R. Muller,et al.  Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. II. Effects of environmental manipulations , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[15]  T. van Groen,et al.  The postsubicular cortex in the rat: characterization of the fourth region of the subicular cortex and its connections , 1990, Brain Research.

[16]  R U Muller,et al.  Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. I. Description and quantitative analysis , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[17]  C. Gallistel The organization of learning , 1990 .

[18]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Dead Reckoning, Landmark Learning, and the Sense of Direction: A Neurophysiological and Computational Hypothesis , 1991, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  J. Taube,et al.  Lesions of the rat postsubiculum impair performance on spatial tasks. , 1992, Behavioral and neural biology.

[20]  H. Shibata Direct projections from the anterior thalamic nuclei to the retrohippocampal region in the rat , 1993, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[21]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Spatial selectivity of unit activity in the hippocampal granular layer , 1993, Hippocampus.

[22]  S. Mizumori,et al.  Directionally selective mnemonic properties of neurons in the lateral dorsal nucleus of the thalamus of rats , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[23]  S. Wiener Spatial and behavioral correlates of striatal neurons in rats performing a self-initiated navigation task , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[24]  B L McNaughton,et al.  Dynamics of the hippocampal ensemble code for space. , 1993, Science.

[25]  C D Salzman,et al.  Neural mechanisms for forming a perceptual decision. , 1994, Science.

[26]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Neurons responding to whole-body motion in the primate hippocampus , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[27]  A Berthoz,et al.  Inertial, Substratal and Landmark Cue Control of Hippocampal CA1 Place Cell Activity , 1995, The European journal of neuroscience.

[28]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Place cells, head direction cells, and the learning of landmark stability , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[29]  J S Taube,et al.  Preferential use of the landmark navigational system by head direction cells in rats. , 1995, Behavioral neuroscience.

[30]  J. Taube Head direction cells recorded in the anterior thalamic nuclei of freely moving rats , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.