Inferences about Effort and Ability

Perceivers received information about a target person’s level of athletic performance (e.g., kicking a soccer ball) and the situational forces surrounding that performance. Perceivers’ inferences about the effort and ability of the target depended on whether the situational forces involved psychological forces (a bribe offered for low performance) or physical forces (a wind that hindered performance). Replicating past research, psychological forces had a stronger impact on inferred ability given the presence of low performance, as opposed to high performance. Additional analyses indicated that this pattern was mediated by inferences about the effort of the target. In contrast, physical forces appeared to have a direct impact on inferences of ability and affected inferences based on both low and high performance. The results suggest that perceivers make inferences about multiple attributes within a target person and, in the process, attempt to reconcile those inferences.

[1]  Y. Trope Dispositional bias in person perception: A hypothesis-testing perspective , 1998 .

[2]  V B CLINE,et al.  INTERPERSONAL PERCEPTION. , 1964, Progress in experimental personality research.

[3]  Joel Cooper,et al.  Attribution and social interaction : the legacy of Edward E. Jones , 1998 .

[4]  D. J. Schneider,et al.  The Effects of Behavioral, Situational, and Person Information on Different Attribution Judgments , 1994 .

[5]  R. Vonk Effects of Outcome Dependency on Correspondence Bias , 1999 .

[6]  M. Brewer,et al.  A schematic model of dispositional attribution in interpersonal perception. , 1979 .

[7]  D. Trafimow Situation-Specific Effects in Person Memory , 1998 .

[8]  D. Trafimow,et al.  Mapping Perfect and Imperfect Duties onto Hierarchically and Partially Restrictive Trait Dimensions , 1999 .

[9]  S. Fiske,et al.  The Handbook of Social Psychology , 1935 .

[10]  S. Read,et al.  Explanatory coherence in social explanations : a parallel distributed processing account , 1993 .

[11]  G. Reeder Dispositional Inferences of Ability: Content and Process , 1997 .

[12]  S. Asch Forming impressions of personality. , 1946, Journal of Abnormal Psychology.

[13]  Donal E. Carlston,et al.  Negativity and extremity biases in impression formation: A review of explanations. , 1989 .

[14]  Ad van Knippenberg,et al.  The Sovereignty of Negative Inferences: Suspicion of Ulterior Motives Does Not Reduce the Negativity Effect , 1994 .

[15]  S. Read,et al.  Constructing causal scenarios: a knowledge structure approach to causal reasoning. , 1987, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[16]  D. Gilbert,et al.  The correspondence bias. , 1995, Psychological bulletin.

[17]  R. Baron,et al.  Toward an Ecological Theory of Social Perception , 1983 .

[18]  D. A. Kenny,et al.  The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[19]  Solomon E. Asch,et al.  THINKING ABOUT PERSONS , 1984 .

[20]  H. Kelley The processes of causal attribution. , 1973 .

[21]  J. McClure Discounting causes of behavior : Are two reasons better than one ? , 1998 .

[22]  J. Neter,et al.  Applied linear statistical models : regression, analysis of variance, and experimental designs , 1974 .

[23]  D. Hilton,et al.  Are goals or preconditions better explanations? It depends on the question , 1998 .

[24]  D. Trafimow The Implications of Success for Hierarchically and Partially Restrictive Ability Dimensions , 1997 .

[25]  Richard P. Larrick,et al.  When one cause casts doubt on another: A normative analysis of discounting in causal attribution. , 1995 .

[26]  Steven Fein Effects of suspicion on attributional thinking and the correspondence bias , 1996 .

[27]  E. E. Jones,et al.  From Acts To Dispositions The Attribution Process In Person Perception1 , 1965 .

[28]  J. H. Fleming,et al.  Multiple-Audience Problems, Tactical Communication, and Social Interaction: A Relational-Regulation Perspective , 1994 .

[29]  S. Read,et al.  Rapist or "Regular Guy": Explanatory Coherence in the Construction of Mental Models of Others , 1993 .

[30]  R. Vonk,et al.  Impression Formation and Impression Management: Motives, Traits, and Likeability Inferred from Self-Promoting and Self-Deprecating Behavior , 1999 .

[31]  Bylaws of Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc , 1975 .

[32]  B. Malle,et al.  How People Explain Behavior: A New Theoretical Framework , 1999, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[33]  N. Anderson,et al.  Performance = Motivation × Ability: An integration-theoretical analysis. , 1974 .

[34]  F. Heider The psychology of interpersonal relations , 1958 .

[35]  B. Malle,et al.  Conceptual structure and social functions of behavior explanations: beyond person--situation attributions. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[36]  D. Erickson,et al.  Inferential Hopscotch: How People Draw Social Inferences From Behavior , 1995 .

[37]  G. Reeder,et al.  The attribution of morality. , 1983 .

[38]  G. Reeder,et al.  When actions speak louder than words: Implicational schemata and the attribution of ability. , 1980 .