Spatial Representation of the Workspace in Blind, Low Vision, and Sighted Human Participants

It has been proposed that haptic spatial perception depends on one’s visual abilities. We tested spatial perception in the workspace using a combination of haptic matching and line drawing tasks. There were 132 participants with varying degrees of visual ability ranging from congenitally blind to normally sighted. Each participant was blindfolded and asked to match a haptic target position felt under a table with their nondominant hand using a pen in their dominant hand. Once the pen was in position on the tabletop, they had to draw a line of equal length to a previously felt reference object by moving the pen laterally. We used targets at three different locations to evaluate whether different starting positions relative to the body give rise to different matching errors, drawn line lengths, or drawn line angles. We found no influence of visual ability on matching error, drawn line length, or line angle, but we found that early-blind participants are slightly less consistent in their matching errors across space. We conclude that the elementary haptic abilities tested in these tasks do not depend on visual experience.

[1]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Matching locations is not just matching sensory representations , 2016, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  C. Spence,et al.  Early Vision Impairs Tactile Perception in the Blind , 2004, Current Biology.

[3]  Alexandre Pouget,et al.  A computational perspective on the neural basis of multisensory spatial representations , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[4]  Jeroen B. J. Smeets,et al.  Errors in visuo-haptic and haptic-haptic location matching are stable over long periods of time. , 2016, Acta psychologica.

[5]  M. Paré,et al.  Early-blind human subjects localize sound sources better than sighted subjects , 1998, Nature.

[6]  C. Seashore,et al.  The comparative sensitiveness of blind and seeing persons. , 1918 .

[7]  D. Sparks,et al.  Sensorimotor integration in the primate superior colliculus. I. Motor convergence. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  Astrid M L Kappers,et al.  Large systematic deviations in a bimanual parallelity task: further analysis of contributing factors. , 2003, Acta psychologica.

[9]  Bill Jones,et al.  Development of Cutaneous and Kinesthetic Localization by Blind and Sighted Children , 1972 .

[10]  Yale E. Cohen,et al.  A common reference frame for movement plans in the posterior parietal cortex , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[11]  G. Sandini,et al.  Impairment of auditory spatial localization in congenitally blind human subjects , 2013, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[12]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Symmetry perception in the blind. , 2010, Acta psychologica.

[13]  Location and Distance Estimates by Blind and Sighted Children , 1975, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[14]  Franco Lepore,et al.  Early- and Late-Onset Blind Individuals Show Supra-Normal Auditory Abilities in Far-Space , 2004, Current Biology.

[15]  David L. Sparks,et al.  Sensori-motor integration in the primate superior colliculus , 1991 .

[16]  Marc O. Ernst,et al.  Path integration in tactile perception of shapes , 2014, Behavioural Brain Research.

[17]  M. Proulx,et al.  Visual experience facilitates allocentric spatial representation , 2013, Behavioural Brain Research.

[18]  Tina Iachini,et al.  Does blindness affect egocentric and allocentric frames of reference in small and large scale spaces? , 2014, Behavioural Brain Research.

[19]  E. Gentaz,et al.  The haptic oblique effect in the perception of rod orientation by blind adults , 1998, Perception & psychophysics.

[20]  R. J. van Beers,et al.  Integration of proprioceptive and visual position-information: An experimentally supported model. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  Astrid M L Kappers,et al.  Haptic Perception of Spatial Relations , 1999, Perception.

[22]  Christian Duriez,et al.  Haptics: Neuroscience, Devices, Modeling, and Applications , 2014, Lecture Notes in Computer Science.

[23]  H. V. Van Mier Effects of visual information regarding allocentric processing in haptic parallelity matching. , 2013, Acta psychologica.

[24]  R L Klatzky,et al.  Spatial and movement-based heuristics for encoding pattern information through touch. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[25]  P. Haggard,et al.  The perceived position of the hand in space , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[26]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Improved auditory spatial tuning in blind humans , 1999, Nature.

[27]  C. Hofsten,et al.  The integration of sensory information in the development of precise manual pointing , 1988, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  Astrid M L Kappers,et al.  Haptic perception of parallelity in the midsagittal plane. , 2002, Acta psychologica.

[29]  Mark Holllns,et al.  Spatial updating in blind and sighted people , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[30]  Astrid M L Kappers,et al.  The contributions of egocentric and allocentric reference frames in haptic spatial tasks. , 2004, Acta psychologica.

[31]  C. Spence,et al.  Developmental vision determines the reference frame for the multisensory control of action , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[32]  M A Heller,et al.  Mechanisms in the haptic horizontal-vertical illusion: Evidence from sighted and blind subjects , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[33]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Proprioceptive Biases in Different Experimental Designs , 2014, EuroHaptics.

[34]  A. Kappers,et al.  Haptic orientation perception benefits from visual experience: Evidence from early-blind, late-blind, and sighted people , 2008, Perception & psychophysics.

[35]  A. Kappers,et al.  Differences between Early-Blind, Late-Blind, and Blindfolded-Sighted People in Haptic Spatial-Configuration Learning and Resulting Memory Traces , 2007, Perception.

[36]  M B Jones,et al.  Length discrimination on the skin. , 1973, The American journal of psychology.

[37]  Edouard Gentaz,et al.  Role of gravitational cues in the haptic perception of orientation , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.

[38]  Luigi F. Cuturi,et al.  Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews , 2022 .

[39]  G. Sandini,et al.  Poor Haptic Orientation Discrimination in Nonsighted Children May Reflect Disruption of Cross-Sensory Calibration , 2010, Current Biology.

[40]  Jennifer L. Milne,et al.  Enhanced auditory spatial localization in blind echolocators , 2015, Neuropsychologia.