High prevalence of prospective memory impairment in the elderly and in early-stage dementia: Findings from a population-based study

Remembering to carry out intended actions is important for the effective performance of daily activities, but generally declines with age. This study aimed to establish the prevalence of prospective memory impairment in the elderly population, describe the age function and identify risk factors for impairment. An event-based prospective memory test was administered to 11,956 participants aged 65 + in the screening stage of a population-based study, the Medical Research Council Cognitive Function and Ageing Study (MRC CFAS). Only 54% of the sample succeeded on the task. Logistic regression analysis showed that successful performance was strongly and linearly related to age, and that male gender, less education and lower social status substantially increased the risk of prospective memory impairment. There was a very high prevalence of prospective memory impairment in 388 individuals with very mild dementia, of whom only 8% succeeded on the task. These findings raise concerns about the well-being and safety of many older people. Copyright © 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Language: en

[1]  P. Rendell,et al.  Aging and prospective memory: differences between naturalistic and laboratory tasks. , 1999, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[2]  B. Ober,et al.  Naturally occurring and experimentally induced tip-of-the-tongue experiences in three adult age groups. , 1999, Psychology and aging.

[3]  M. Moscovitch A Neuropsychological Approach to Perception and Memory in Normal and Pathological Aging , 1982 .

[4]  T. Tombaugh,et al.  The Mini‐Mental State Examination: A Comprehensive Review , 1992, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[5]  Roger W. Morrell,et al.  Prospective memory and aging: The effects of working memory and prospective memory task load , 1997 .

[6]  J. Ellis Prospective memory or the realization of delayed intentions: A conceptual framework for research. , 1996 .

[7]  M. McDaniel,et al.  The bizarreness effect: it's not surprising, it's complex. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[8]  John R. Crawford,et al.  The Executive Decline Hypothesis of Cognitive Aging: Do Executive Deficits Qualify as Differential Deficits and Do They Mediate Age-Related Memory Decline? , 2000 .

[9]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Age-related deficits in prospective memory: the influence of task complexity. , 1992, Psychology and aging.

[10]  T. Mäntylä,et al.  Remembering to remember in adulthood: A population-based study on aging and prospective memory , 1997 .

[11]  M. McGee,et al.  The description of activities of daily living in five centres in England and Wales. Medical Research Council Cognitive Function and Ageing Study. , 1998, Age and ageing.

[12]  A. Smith,et al.  Rapidly progressing atrophy of medial temporal lobe in Alzheimer's disease , 1994, The Lancet.

[13]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.

[14]  Morris Moscovitch,et al.  The neuropsychology of memory and aging. , 1992 .

[15]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Normal aging and prospective memory. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[16]  C. Brayne,et al.  CAMCOG--a concise neuropsychological test to assist dementia diagnosis: socio-demographic determinants in an elderly population sample. , 1995, The British journal of clinical psychology.

[17]  K. Cherry,et al.  Age and individual differences influence prospective memory , 1999 .

[18]  伊藤 秀一 Poverty in the United Kingdom , 1999 .

[19]  A. Smith,et al.  Use of structural imaging to study the progression of Alzheimer's disease. , 1996, British medical bulletin.

[20]  Elizabeth A. Maylor,et al.  Aging and forgetting in prospective and retrospective memory tasks. , 1993, Psychology and aging.

[21]  M Schemper,et al.  Explained variation for logistic regression. , 1996, Statistics in medicine.

[22]  M. Folstein,et al.  Limits of the ‘Mini-Mental State’ as a screening test for dementia and delirium among hospital patients , 1982, Psychological Medicine.

[23]  F. Huppert,et al.  Prospective memory impairment as an early indicator of dementia. , 1993, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[24]  E. Maylor,et al.  Age and Prospective Memory , 1990, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[25]  N. L. Bowles,et al.  On subject selection in cognitive aging research. , 1984, Experimental aging research.

[26]  Elizabeth A. Maylor,et al.  Changes in Event-Based Prospective Memory Across Adulthood , 1998 .

[27]  Mark A. McDaniel,et al.  Aging and prospective memory: examining the influences of self-initiated retrieval processes. , 1995 .

[28]  A. Wilkins,et al.  What brings intentions to mind? An in situ study of prospective memory. , 1997, Memory.

[29]  D. A. Walsh,et al.  Orienting task effects on EDR and free recall in three age groups. , 1978, Journal of gerontology.

[30]  B. G. Rule,et al.  Prospective memory and self-reports of memory abilities in older adults. , 1987, Canadian journal of psychology.

[31]  C B Mayhorn,et al.  Effect of age on event-based and time-based prospective memory. , 1997, Psychology and aging.

[32]  L. Kvavilashvili Remembering intention as a distinct form of memory , 1987 .

[33]  J. Copeland,et al.  A computerized psychiatric diagnostic system and case nomenclature for elderly subjects: GMS and AGECAT , 1986, Psychological Medicine.