Dissociation between intentional and incidental sequence learning in Huntington's disease.

The ability to acquire and act upon serial order information is fundamental to almost all forms of adaptive behaviour. There is growing evidence that such knowledge may be acquired through a number of different means, each perhaps with its own neuronal substrate. One major distinction is between serial order information acquired intentionally and leading to explicit conscious knowledge of the sequence structure, and information acquired incidentally through experience. While this latter form of knowledge influences behaviour, it may do so without the participant being aware of the sequential information, i.e. it is acquired implicitly. Evidence from physiological and lesion studies in animals and imaging studies in humans suggests that these two forms of learning may have dissociable neuronal substrates. Specifically, the striato-thalamo-cortical circuit centred on the caudate nucleus is proposed to be involved in intentional sequence learning and that based on the putamen on incidental learning. The present study tested one part of this proposed dissociation by assessing patients with Huntington's disease on tasks of the two forms of learning. On the test of trial-and-error intentional learning there were marked deficits, which were closely related to disease progression and to measures of executive cognitive dysfunction. This finding was in contrast to the finding from the incidental learning task. Performance of the Huntington's disease group was essentially normal and unrelated to measures of disease progression and cognitive status. The results, although supportive of the proposed dual-system hypothesis, offer only partial confirmation. Further direct study is required using similar tasks in patients with putamenal disorder or lesions within the skeletomotor striato-thalamo-cortical circuit.

[1]  A. Graybiel,et al.  Distributed but convergent ordering of corticostriatal projections: analysis of the frontal eye field and the supplementary eye field in the macaque monkey , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[2]  J. Crawford,et al.  Cognitive performance in UK sample of presymptomatic people carrying the gene for Huntington's disease. , 1995, Journal of medical genetics.

[3]  M. Nissen,et al.  Procedural learning is impaired in Huntington's disease: Evidence from the serial reaction time task , 1991, Neuropsychologia.

[4]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Transition of Brain Activation from Frontal to Parietal Areas in Visuomotor Sequence Learning , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  R. Hichwa,et al.  Positron emission tomographic scan investigations of Huntington's disease: Cerebral metabolic correlates of cognitive function , 1988, Annals of neurology.

[6]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.

[7]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Attention and stimulus characteristics determine the locus of motor-sequence encoding. A PET study. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[8]  H Mushiake,et al.  Pallidal neuron activity during sequential arm movements. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  M. Ghilardi,et al.  Patterns of regional brain activation associated with different forms of motor learning , 2000, Brain Research.

[10]  D. Weinberger,et al.  Prefrontal cortical blood flow and cognitive function in Huntington's disease. , 1988, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[11]  H. Nelson A Modified Card Sorting Test Sensitive to Frontal Lobe Defects , 1976, Cortex.

[12]  J. Joseph,et al.  Activity in the caudate nucleus of monkey during spatial sequencing. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  A. Parent,et al.  The current model of basal ganglia organization under scrutiny , 1998, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[14]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Anatomy of motor learning. II. Subcortical structures and learning by trial and error. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  J C Rothwell,et al.  Simple and complex movements in a patient with infarction of the right supplementary motor area , 1986, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[16]  I. Shoulson,et al.  Huntington disease , 1981, Neurology.

[17]  D. Balota,et al.  Implicit Memory and the Formation of New Associations in Nondemented Parkinson′s Disease Individuals and Individuals with Senile Dementia of the Alzheimer Type: A Serial Reaction Time (SRT) Investigation , 1993, Brain and Cognition.

[18]  M. Hallett,et al.  Dynamic cortical involvement in implicit and explicit motor sequence learning. A PET study. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[19]  J. Stroop Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .

[20]  J. Doyon,et al.  Role of the Striatum, Cerebellum, and Frontal Lobes in the Learning of a Visuomotor Sequence , 1997, Brain and Cognition.

[21]  C. Marsden,et al.  Disturbance of sequential movements in patients with Parkinson's disease. , 1987, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[22]  S. Folstein,et al.  Early Loss of Neostriatal Striosome Neurons in Huntington's Disease , 1995, Journal of neuropathology and experimental neurology.

[23]  T. Robbins,et al.  Cognitive functions and corticostriatal circuits: insights from Huntington's disease , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[24]  A. Georgopoulos,et al.  Motor cortical encoding of serial order in a context-recall task. , 1999, Science.

[25]  Kae Nakamura,et al.  Neuronal activity in medial frontal cortex during learning of sequential procedures. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  S. Rauch,et al.  Striatal recruitment during an implicit sequence learning task as measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging , 1997, Human brain mapping.

[27]  Peter Dayan,et al.  A Neural Substrate of Prediction and Reward , 1997, Science.

[28]  A. Graybiel Building action repertoires: memory and learning functions of the basal ganglia , 1995, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[29]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Differential roles of monkey striatum in learning of sequential hand movement , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[30]  A. Cooper,et al.  The selective vulnerability of striatopallidal neurons , 1999, Progress in Neurobiology.

[31]  D. Eidelberg,et al.  Book Review Movement Disorders: Neurologic principles and practice Edited by Ray L. Watts and William C. Koller. 779 pp., illustrated. New York, McGraw-Hill, 1997. $125. 0-07-035203-8 , 1997 .

[32]  M. Inase,et al.  Neuronal activity in the primate premotor, supplementary, and precentral motor cortex during visually guided and internally determined sequential movements. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Corticocortical connections of area F3 (SMA‐proper) and area F6 (pre‐SMA) in the macaque monkey , 1993, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[34]  K. Berridge,et al.  Implementation of Action Sequences by a Neostriatal Site: A Lesion Mapping Study of Grooming Syntax , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  P. Strick,et al.  Preferential activity of dentate neurons during limb movements guided by vision. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  G. E. Alexander Basal Ganglia‐Thalamocortical Circuits: Their Role in Control of Movements , 1994, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[37]  A. Graybiel,et al.  Role of [corrected] nigrostriatal dopamine system in learning to perform sequential motor tasks in a predictive manner. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  G. Pearlson,et al.  Longitudinal change in basal ganglia volume in patients with Huntington's disease , 1997, Neurology.

[39]  K. Doya,et al.  Parallel neural networks for learning sequential procedures , 1999, Trends in Neurosciences.

[40]  W Grodd,et al.  Impaired procedural learning after damage to the left supplementary motor area (SMA). , 1996, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[41]  M. Folstein,et al.  Population-based norms for the Mini-Mental State Examination by age and educational level. , 1993, JAMA.

[42]  T. Robbins,et al.  The relationship between striatal dopamine receptor binding and cognitive performance in Huntington's disease. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[43]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  A PET investigation of implicit and explicit sequence learning , 1995 .

[44]  M. Hallett,et al.  Procedural learning in Parkinson's disease and cerebellar degeneration , 1993, Annals of neurology.

[45]  Jane S. Paulsen,et al.  Unified Huntington's disease rating scale: Reliability and consistency , 1996, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[46]  R. Passingham,et al.  The Time Course of Changes during Motor Sequence Learning: A Whole-Brain fMRI Study , 1998, NeuroImage.

[47]  松本 直幸,et al.  Role of Nigrostriatal Dopamine System in Learning to Perform Sequential Motor Tasks in a Predictive Manner , 2000 .

[48]  Mark Hallett,et al.  Learning in Parkinson’s disease: eyeblink conditioning, declarative learning, and procedural learning , 1999, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[49]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Activation of human presupplementary motor area in learning of sequential procedures: a functional MRI study. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[50]  K. Leenders,et al.  Bradykinesia in early Huntington’s disease , 2000, Neurology.

[51]  A. Cools,et al.  Evidence for a role of the caudate nucleus in the sequential organization of behaviour , 1982, Behavioural Brain Research.

[52]  M. Nissen,et al.  Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.

[53]  Irene Daum,et al.  Sequence learning in Parkinson’s disease: a comparison of spatial-attention and number-response sequences , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[54]  B. Milner,et al.  Interhemispheric differences in the localization of psychological processes in man. , 1971, British medical bulletin.

[55]  E. Siemers,et al.  Rate of functional decline in Huntington’s disease , 2000, Neurology.

[56]  L. Henderson,et al.  Serial reaction time learning and Parkinson's disease: Evidence for a procedural learning deficit , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[57]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Functional Anatomy of Visuomotor Skill Learning in Human Subjects Examined with Positron Emission Tomography , 1996, The European journal of neuroscience.

[58]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Functional Mapping of Sequence Learning in Normal Humans , 1995, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[59]  J. F. Dormont,et al.  Effects of caudate nucleus cooling on the performance of conditioned movements in cats , 1979, Neuroscience Letters.

[60]  M. Nissen,et al.  Implicit learning in patients with probable Alzheimer's disease , 1987, Neurology.

[61]  E. Procyk,et al.  Anterior cingulate activity during routine and non-routine sequential behaviors in macaques , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[62]  D. Brooks,et al.  Motor sequence learning: a study with positron emission tomography , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[63]  R. Siegert,et al.  Implicit learning in Parkinson's disease: evidence from a verbal version of the serial reaction time task. , 1998, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[64]  Daniel B. Willingham,et al.  Evidence for dissociable motor skills in Huntington’s disease patients , 1993, Psychobiology.

[65]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Learning of sequential movements in the monkey: process of learning and retention of memory. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[66]  V. Kostic,et al.  Visuomotor skill learning on serial reaction time task in patients with early Parkinson's disease , 2000, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[67]  H. Herzog,et al.  Cortical and subcortical glucose consumption measured by PET in patients with Huntington's disease. , 1990, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[68]  S P Wise,et al.  Distributed modular architectures linking basal ganglia, cerebellum, and cerebral cortex: their role in planning and controlling action. , 1995, Cerebral cortex.