Posture influences motor imagery: An fMRI study

Motor imagery is widely used to study cognitive aspects of the neural control of action. However, what is exactly simulated during motor imagery is still a matter of debate. On the one hand, it is conceivable that motor imagery is an embodied cognitive process, involving a simulation of movements of one's own body. The alternative possibility is that, although motor imagery relies on knowledge of the motor processes, it does not entail an actual motor simulation that is influenced by the physical configuration of one's own body. Here we discriminate between these two hypotheses, in the context of an established motor imagery task: laterality judgments of rotated hand drawings. We found that reaction times of hand laterality judgments followed the biomechanical constraints of left or right hand movements. Crucially, the position of subjects' own left and right arm influenced laterality judgments of left and right hands. In neural terms, hand laterality judgments activated a parieto-frontal network. The activity within this network increased with increasing biomechanical complexity of the imagined hand movements, even when the amount of stimulus rotation was identical. Moreover, activity in the intraparietal sulcus was modulated by subjects' own hand position: a larger incongruence in orientation between the subjects' hand and the stimulus hand led to a selective increase in intraparietal activity. Our results indicate that motor imagery generates motor plans that depend on the current configuration of the limbs. This motor plan is calculated by a parieto-frontal network. Within this network, the posterior parietal cortex appears to incorporate proprioceptive information related to the current position of the body into the motor plan.

[1]  Alessio Toraldo,et al.  Dissociation between the mental rotation of visual images and motor images in unilateral brain-damaged patients , 2003, Brain and Cognition.

[2]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Mental state inference using visual control parameters. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[3]  P. Viviani,et al.  A developmental study of the relationship between geometry and kinematics in drawing movements. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[4]  H. Branch Coslett,et al.  Mental motor imagery and the body schema: evidence for proprioceptive dominance , 2004, Neuroscience Letters.

[5]  A. Wohlschlager,et al.  Action generation and action perception in imitation : An instance of the ideomotor principle , 2003 .

[6]  Ivan Toni,et al.  Neural Topography and Content of Movement Representations , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[7]  L. Craighero,et al.  The influence of hand posture on corticospinal excitability during motor imagery: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[8]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[9]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A unified statistical approach for determining significant signals in images of cerebral activation , 1996, Human brain mapping.

[10]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A critique of functional localisers , 2006, NeuroImage.

[11]  A. Schleicher,et al.  High‐resolution MRI reflects myeloarchitecture and cytoarchitecture of human cerebral cortex , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[12]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Parieto-frontal coding of reaching: an integrated framework , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  M S Gazzaniga,et al.  Cerebrally Lateralized Mental Representations of Hand Shape and Movement , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  A. Goldman,et al.  Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[15]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Role of the posterior parietal cortex in updating reaching movements to a visual target , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[16]  H. Coslett,et al.  Pain and the body schema: evidence for peripheral effects on mental representations of movement. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[17]  J B Poline,et al.  Partially overlapping neural networks for real and imagined hand movements. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[18]  Ivan Toni,et al.  Neural correlates of the chronic fatigue syndrome--an fMRI study. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[19]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[20]  R. Andersen,et al.  Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex and its use in planning movements. , 1997, Annual review of neuroscience.

[21]  R. Passingham,et al.  Action observation and acquired motor skills: an FMRI study with expert dancers. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[22]  L. Parsons Temporal and kinematic properties of motor behavior reflected in mentally simulated action. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[23]  R. Passingham,et al.  Temporary interference in human lateral premotor cortex suggests dominance for the selection of movements. A study using transcranial magnetic stimulation. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[24]  L. Parsons Imagined spatial transformations of one's hands and feet , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.

[25]  E. Cabanis,et al.  The Human Brain: Surface, Three-Dimensional Sectional Anatomy and Mri , 1991 .

[26]  Paul B. Johnson,et al.  Cortical networks for visual reaching: physiological and anatomical organization of frontal and parietal lobe arm regions. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[27]  R. Ivry,et al.  Ipsilateral motor cortex activity during unimanual hand movements relates to task complexity. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  Scott H. Johnson,et al.  Imagining the impossible: intact motor representations in hemiplegics , 2000, Neuroreport.

[29]  Zoubin Ghahramani,et al.  Computational principles of movement neuroscience , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[30]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  The motor theory of social cognition: a critique , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[31]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Systematic changes in motor cortex cell activity with arm posture during directional isometric force generation. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  S. Scott,et al.  Reaching movements with similar hand paths but different arm orientations. II. Activity of individual cells in dorsal premotor cortex and parietal area 5. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  J. Ashburner,et al.  Multimodal Image Coregistration and Partitioning—A Unified Framework , 1997, NeuroImage.

[34]  S. H. Johnson,et al.  Intact Motor Imagery in Chronic Upper Limb Hemiplegics: Evidence for Activity-Independent Action Representations , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[35]  M. Jeannerod The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[36]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Selective Activation of a Parietofrontal Circuit during Implicitly Imagined Prehension , 2002, NeuroImage.

[37]  K. Sekiyama,et al.  Kinesthetic aspects of mental representations in the identification of left and right hands , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.

[38]  M. Jeannerod Neural Simulation of Action: A Unifying Mechanism for Motor Cognition , 2001, NeuroImage.

[39]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Mental rotation of objects versus hands: neural mechanisms revealed by positron emission tomography. , 1998, Psychophysiology.

[40]  V. Gallese The manifold nature of interpersonal relations: the quest for a common mechanism. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[41]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  A unifying view of the basis of social cognition , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[42]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Parietal Lobe: From Action Organization to Intention Understanding , 2005, Science.

[43]  J. Ashburner,et al.  Nonlinear spatial normalization using basis functions , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[44]  P. Fitts,et al.  INFORMATION CAPACITY OF DISCRETE MOTOR RESPONSES. , 1964, Journal of experimental psychology.

[45]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  How Many Subjects Constitute a Study? , 1999, NeuroImage.

[46]  Carlo Umiltà,et al.  Selective Deficit of Imagining Finger Configurations , 2001, Cortex.

[47]  Jesper Andersson,et al.  Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic , 2005, NeuroImage.

[48]  S. Scott,et al.  Reaching movements with similar hand paths but different arm orientations. I. Activity of individual cells in motor cortex. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[49]  R. E Passingham,et al.  Cerebral dominance for action in the human brain: the selection of actions , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[50]  A. Sirigu,et al.  Left and right hand recognition in upper limb amputees. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[51]  A. Sirigu,et al.  Motor and Visual Imagery as Two Complementary but Neurally Dissociable Mental Processes , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[52]  S. Aglioti,et al.  Selective impairment of hand mental rotation in patients with focal hand dystonia. , 2005, Brain : a journal of neurology.