The Influence of Spatiotemporal Structure of Noisy Stimuli in Decision Making

Decision making is a process of utmost importance in our daily lives, the study of which has been receiving notable attention for decades. Nevertheless, the neural mechanisms underlying decision making are still not fully understood. Computational modeling has revealed itself as a valuable asset to address some of the fundamental questions. Biophysically plausible models, in particular, are useful in bridging the different levels of description that experimental studies provide, from the neural spiking activity recorded at the cellular level to the performance reported at the behavioral level. In this article, we have reviewed some of the recent progress made in the understanding of the neural mechanisms that underlie decision making. We have performed a critical evaluation of the available results and address, from a computational perspective, aspects of both experimentation and modeling that so far have eluded comprehension. To guide the discussion, we have selected a central theme which revolves around the following question: how does the spatiotemporal structure of sensory stimuli affect the perceptual decision-making process? This question is a timely one as several issues that still remain unresolved stem from this central theme. These include: (i) the role of spatiotemporal input fluctuations in perceptual decision making, (ii) how to extend the current results and models derived from two-alternative choice studies to scenarios with multiple competing evidences, and (iii) to establish whether different types of spatiotemporal input fluctuations affect decision-making outcomes in distinctive ways. And although we have restricted our discussion mostly to visual decisions, our main conclusions are arguably generalizable; hence, their possible extension to other sensory modalities is one of the points in our discussion.

[1]  R. Stein A THEORETICAL ANALYSIS OF NEURONAL VARIABILITY. , 1965, Biophysical journal.

[2]  E. Rolls,et al.  Decision‐making and Weber's law: a neurophysiological model , 2006, The European journal of neuroscience.

[3]  Verónica Nácher,et al.  Neural correlates of a postponed decision report , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[4]  Bingni W. Brunton,et al.  Rats and Humans Can Optimally Accumulate Evidence for Decision-Making , 2013, Science.

[5]  R. Romo,et al.  Temporal Evolution of a Decision-Making Process in Medial Premotor Cortex , 2002, Neuron.

[6]  Alexander C. Huk,et al.  Temporal Dynamics Underlying Perceptual Decision Making: Insights from the Interplay between an Attractor Model and Parietal Neurophysiology , 2008, Front. Neurosci..

[7]  A. Pouget,et al.  Not Noisy, Just Wrong: The Role of Suboptimal Inference in Behavioral Variability , 2012, Neuron.

[8]  David J. Freedman,et al.  Categorical representation of visual stimuli in the primate prefrontal cortex. , 2001, Science.

[9]  K. H. Britten,et al.  Responses of neurons in macaque MT to stochastic motion signals , 1993, Visual Neuroscience.

[10]  A J Ahumada,et al.  Model of human visual-motion sensing. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[11]  Timothy D. Hanks,et al.  Bounded Integration in Parietal Cortex Underlies Decisions Even When Viewing Duration Is Dictated by the Environment , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  Clarissa A. Thompson,et al.  Children are not like older adults: a diffusion model analysis of developmental changes in speeded responses. , 2012, Child development.

[13]  O. Braddick,et al.  What is Noise for the Motion System? , 1996, Vision Research.

[14]  Jochen Ditterich,et al.  Perceptual Decisions between Multiple Directions of Visual Motion , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  V. Mountcastle,et al.  NEURAL ACTIVITY IN MECHANORECEPTIVE CUTANEOUS AFFERENTS: STIMULUS-RESPONSE RELATIONS, WEBER FUNCTIONS, AND INFORMATION TRANSMISSION. , 1965, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  A. Tversky,et al.  Context-dependent preferences , 1993 .

[17]  N Brunel,et al.  Correlations of cortical Hebbian reverberations: theory versus experiment , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[18]  W. E. Hick Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , 1948, Nature.

[19]  D. Heeger Normalization of cell responses in cat striate cortex , 1992, Visual Neuroscience.

[20]  K. H. Britten,et al.  A relationship between behavioral choice and the visual responses of neurons in macaque MT , 1996, Visual Neuroscience.

[21]  Xiao-Jing Wang,et al.  A Recurrent Network Mechanism of Time Integration in Perceptual Decisions , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  Emilio Salinas,et al.  So Many Choices: What Computational Models Reveal about Decision-Making Mechanisms , 2008, Neuron.

[23]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .

[24]  Xiao-Jing Wang,et al.  Similarity Effect and Optimal Control of Multiple-Choice Decision Making , 2008, Neuron.

[25]  A. Pouget,et al.  Variance as a Signature of Neural Computations during Decision Making , 2011, Neuron.

[26]  Marian Stamp Dawkins,et al.  The Noisy Brain: Stochastic Dynamics as a Principle of Brain Function The Noisy Brain: Stochastic Dynamics as a Principle of Brain Function. By Edmund T. Rolls & Gustavo Deco. Oxford: Oxford University Press (2010). Pp. 310. Price £37.95 hardback. , 2010, Animal Behaviour.

[27]  Andrew Heathcote,et al.  An integrated model of choices and response times in absolute identification. , 2008, Psychological review.

[28]  Adam Kepecs,et al.  The sniff as a unit of olfactory processing. , 2006, Chemical senses.

[29]  E. Sober From a biological point of view: Let's razor Ockham's razor , 1994 .

[30]  Changes in time scale and sensitivity in Limulus ommatidia , 2004, Documenta Ophthalmologica.

[31]  R. Romo,et al.  Timing and neural encoding of somatosensory parametric working memory in macaque prefrontal cortex. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[32]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Response of Neurons in the Lateral Intraparietal Area during a Combined Visual Discrimination Reaction Time Task , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  J. Ditterich Evidence for time‐variant decision making , 2006, The European journal of neuroscience.

[34]  Xiao-Jing Wang,et al.  Probabilistic Decision Making by Slow Reverberation in Cortical Circuits , 2002, Neuron.

[35]  Francis Tuerlinckx,et al.  Fitting the ratcliff diffusion model to experimental data , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[36]  K. D. Valois,et al.  Motion-reversal Reveals Two Motion Mechanisms Functioning in Scotopic Vision , 1997, Vision Research.

[37]  J. Gold,et al.  Representation of a perceptual decision in developing oculomotor commands , 2000, Nature.

[38]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The physics of optimal decision making: a formal analysis of models of performance in two-alternative forced-choice tasks. , 2006, Psychological review.

[39]  Daniel J. Amit,et al.  Modeling brain function: the world of attractor neural networks, 1st Edition , 1989 .

[40]  Philip L. Smith From Poisson shot noise to the integrated Ornstein–Uhlenbeck process: Neurally principled models of information accumulation in decision-making and response time , 2010 .

[41]  V. Mountcastle,et al.  The sense of flutter-vibration: comparison of the human capacity with response patterns of mechanoreceptive afferents from the monkey hand. , 1968, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  W. Bair,et al.  Correlated Firing in Macaque Visual Area MT: Time Scales and Relationship to Behavior , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  Marius Usher,et al.  Extending a biologically inspired model of choice: multi-alternatives, nonlinearity and value-based multidimensional choice , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[44]  C. Acuña,et al.  Neural Correlates of Decisions and Their Outcomes in the Ventral Premotor Cortex , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  J. Movshon,et al.  The analysis of visual motion: a comparison of neuronal and psychophysical performance , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[46]  Hatim A. Zariwala,et al.  Neural correlates, computation and behavioural impact of decision confidence , 2008, Nature.

[47]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Decision-making with multiple alternatives , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[48]  A. Hodgkin,et al.  Changes in time scale and sensitivity in the ommatidia of Limulus , 1964, The Journal of physiology.

[49]  E. Seidemann,et al.  Temporal gating of neural signals during performance of a visual discrimination task , 1998, Nature.

[50]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Changing your mind: a computational mechanism of vacillation , 2009, Nature.

[51]  R. Romo,et al.  Neuronal correlates of decision-making in secondary somatosensory cortex , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[52]  J. Movshon,et al.  A computational analysis of the relationship between neuronal and behavioral responses to visual motion , 1996, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[53]  R. Romo,et al.  Frequency discrimination in the sense of flutter: psychophysical measurements correlated with postcentral events in behaving monkeys , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[54]  T. Manabe,et al.  Mice Lacking M2 and M3 Muscarinic Acetylcholine Receptors Are Devoid of Cholinergic Smooth Muscle Contractions But Still Viable , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[55]  R. Vogels,et al.  Effects of Category Learning on the Stimulus Selectivity of Macaque References , 2022 .

[56]  P. Glimcher Indeterminacy in brain and behavior. , 2005, Annual review of psychology.

[57]  J. Townsend,et al.  Decision field theory: a dynamic-cognitive approach to decision making in an uncertain environment. , 1993, Psychological review.

[58]  Gustavo Deco,et al.  Stochastic dynamics as a principle of brain function , 2009, Progress in Neurobiology.

[59]  Jochen Ditterich,et al.  New advances in understanding decisions among multiple alternatives , 2012, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[60]  KongFatt Wong-Lin,et al.  Neural Circuit Dynamics Underlying Accumulation of Time-Varying Evidence During Perceptual Decision Making , 2007, Frontiers Comput. Neurosci..

[61]  B. Cumming,et al.  Decision-related activity in sensory neurons reflects more than a neuron’s causal effect , 2009, Nature.

[62]  Jochen Ditterich,et al.  Stochastic models of decisions about motion direction: Behavior and physiology , 2006, Neural Networks.

[63]  M. A. Basso,et al.  Modulation of Neuronal Activity in Superior Colliculus by Changes in Target Probability , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[64]  Barton Childs,et al.  A Biological Point of View. , 1980 .

[65]  Anil Bollimunta,et al.  Local computation of decision-relevant net sensory evidence in parietal cortex. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[66]  Ariel Zylberberg,et al.  The construction of confidence in a perceptual decision , 2012, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[67]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Representation of Confidence Associated with a Decision by Neurons in the Parietal Cortex , 2009, Science.

[68]  R. Romo,et al.  Sensing without Touching Psychophysical Performance Based on Cortical Microstimulation , 2000, Neuron.

[69]  S. Treue,et al.  The response of neurons in areas V1 and MT of the alert rhesus monkey to moving random dot patterns , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[70]  Timothy D. Hanks,et al.  Probabilistic Population Codes for Bayesian Decision Making , 2008, Neuron.

[71]  P. Glimcher,et al.  Reward Value-Based Gain Control: Divisive Normalization in Parietal Cortex , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[72]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  The Diffusion Decision Model: Theory and Data for Two-Choice Decision Tasks , 2008, Neural Computation.

[73]  N. Sigala,et al.  Visual categorization shapes feature selectivity in the primate temporal cortex , 2002, Nature.

[74]  Aaron R. Seitz,et al.  What a difference a parameter makes: A psychophysical comparison of random dot motion algorithms , 2009, Vision Research.

[75]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Loss aversion and inhibition in dynamical models of multialternative choice. , 2004, Psychological review.

[76]  A. Tversky Elimination by aspects: A theory of choice. , 1972 .

[77]  Jochen Ditterich,et al.  A Comparison between Mechanisms of Multi-Alternative Perceptual Decision Making: Ability to Explain Human Behavior, Predictions for Neurophysiology, and Relationship with Decision Theory , 2010, Front. Neurosci..

[78]  W. Newsome,et al.  Microstimulation in visual area MT: effects on direction discrimination performance , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[79]  Anders Ledberg,et al.  Neurobiological Models of Two-Choice Decision Making Can Be Reduced to a One-Dimensional Nonlinear Diffusion Equation , 2008, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[80]  R A Andersen,et al.  The response of area MT and V1 neurons to transparent motion , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[81]  Kenji Morita Dynamical foundations of the neural circuit for bayesian decision making. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[82]  David J. Freedman,et al.  Dynamic population coding of category information in inferior temporal and prefrontal cortex. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[83]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  Diffusive Information Accumulation by Minimal Recurrent Neural Models of Decision Making , 2011, Neural Computation.

[84]  E H Adelson,et al.  Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[85]  Stefano Fusi,et al.  Learning selective top-down control enhances performance in a visual categorization task. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[86]  R. Romo,et al.  Periodicity and Firing Rate As Candidate Neural Codes for the Frequency of Vibrotactile Stimuli , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[87]  K. H. Britten,et al.  Optic flow signals in extrastriate area MST: comparison of perceptual and neuronal sensitivity. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[88]  Xiao-Jing Wang,et al.  Erratum to: Effects of neuromodulation in a cortical network model of object working memory dominated by recurrent inhibition , 2014, Journal of Computational Neuroscience.

[89]  Larissa Albantakis,et al.  Changes of Mind in an Attractor Network of Decision-Making , 2011, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[90]  Gustavo Deco,et al.  Learning to Attend: Modeling the Shaping of Selectivity in Infero-temporal Cortex in a Categorization Task , 2006, Biological Cybernetics.

[91]  R. Romo,et al.  Dynamics of Cortical Neuronal Ensembles Transit from Decision Making to Storage for Later Report , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[92]  V B Mountcastle,et al.  Neural Basis of the Sense of Flutter-Vibration , 1967, Science.

[93]  D. Vickers,et al.  Evidence for an accumulator model of psychophysical discrimination. , 1970, Ergonomics.

[94]  C. Acuña,et al.  Discrimination of line orientation in humans and monkeys. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[95]  J. Townsend,et al.  Multialternative Decision Field Theory: A Dynamic Connectionist Model of Decision Making , 2001 .

[96]  Gustavo Deco,et al.  A Fluctuation-Driven Mechanism for Slow Decision Processes in Reverberant Networks , 2008, PloS one.

[97]  Stefan Treue,et al.  Seeing multiple directions of motion—physiology and psychophysics , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[98]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Neural Activity in Macaque Parietal Cortex Reflects Temporal Integration of Visual Motion Signals during Perceptual Decision Making , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[99]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  A comparison of sequential sampling models for two-choice reaction time. , 2004, Psychological review.

[100]  Larissa Albantakis,et al.  The encoding of alternatives in multiple-choice decision-making , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[101]  W. Newsome,et al.  Neural basis of a perceptual decision in the parietal cortex (area LIP) of the rhesus monkey. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.