Dominance affects determiner selection in language production

Abstract Janssen and Caramazza (2003) show that when producing diminutives or plurals in Dutch, determiner information about the corresponding (singular) base form is active. This is reflected in a time cost for producing the plural or the diminutive with a gender-marked determiner when these forms and the corresponding singular or base form require different determiners. No such cost is observed when singular and plural or base form and diminutive require the same determiner. In a series of picture naming experiments we show that this competition effect is modulated by the relative dominance of the morphological forms. The results can be explained within an extension of the “primed unitised activation account” proposed by Alario and Caramazza (2002) .

[1]  H. Schriefers Syntactic processes in the production of noun phrases , 1993 .

[2]  Herbert Schriefers,et al.  Determiner selection in noun phrase production. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[3]  Inbal Arnon Grammatical Gender , 2022 .

[4]  J. V. Berkum,et al.  Syntactic processes in speech production: the retrieval of grammatical gender , 1997, Cognition.

[5]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Grammatical feature selection in noun phrase production: Evidence from German and Dutch , 2003 .

[6]  D. Geeraerts,et al.  De semantische structuur van het diminutief , 1993 .

[7]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  The selection of closed-class words in noun phrase production: The case of Dutch determiners , 2003 .

[8]  W. La Heij,et al.  The gender-congruency effect in picture-word tasks , 1998 .

[9]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  The production of determiners: evidence from French , 2002, Cognition.

[10]  Kathryn Bock,et al.  Language production : Grammatical encoding , 1994 .

[11]  Geert Booij,et al.  Morfologie. De woordstructuur van het Nederlands , 1998 .

[12]  A Caramazza,et al.  The selection of determiners in noun phrase production. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[13]  W. Levelt Models of word production , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[14]  Zenzi M. Griffin,et al.  The persistence of structural priming: transient activation or implicit learning? , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[15]  R. Baayen,et al.  Singulars and plurals in Dutch: Evidence for a parallel dual-route model , 1997 .

[16]  D. Balota,et al.  The locus of word-frequency effects in the pronunciation task: Lexical access and/or production? ☆ , 1985 .

[17]  J J van Berkum,et al.  Syntactic processes in speech production: the retrieval of grammatical gender. , 1997, Cognition.

[18]  H Schriefers,et al.  Grammatical gender in noun phrase production: the gender interference effect in German. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[19]  Willem J. M. Levelt,et al.  A theory of lexical access in speech production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[20]  G. Dell,et al.  Speech errors, phonotactic constraints, and implicit learning: a study of the role of experience in language production. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[21]  R. H. Baayen,et al.  The CELEX Lexical Database (CD-ROM) , 1996 .

[22]  Wim de Haas,et al.  Morfologisch handboek van het Nederlands : een overzicht van de woordvorming , 1993 .

[23]  Herbert Schriefers,et al.  Selection of gender-marked morphemes in speech production. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.