Regular physical activity improves executive function during task switching in young adults.

The relationship between physical activity levels and executive control functioning was examined in 40 young adults (mean age = 21.4 yrs; 19 females) who were grouped on the basis of their regular physical activity level. Participants performed a task switching paradigm with two conditions. The pure task condition required repeated performance on a single task (e.g., AAAAAA...); the mixed-task condition required participants to change rapidly between different tasks (e.g., AABBAA...). The mixed-task condition created greater executive control requirements due to working memory demands for the maintenance of multiple task sets in memory (compared to the pure task; i.e., mixing cost) and due to requisite inhibition of a task set on switch trials (versus non-switch trials; i.e., switch costs). Task performance measures of reaction times (RTs) and the P3 component of an event-related brain potential were collected. Results show a smaller mixing cost on RTs and P3 amplitudes and a smaller switch cost on RTs for the active group relative to the sedentary group. These data suggest that when the task requires greater amounts of executive control, the physically active group demonstrates a more efficient executive functioning than the sedentary group. Thus, this research presents evidence that regular physical activity selectively improves executive function, as represented by the task switching paradigm, even during young adulthood.

[1]  C. Hillman,et al.  Cardiorespiratory fitness and acute aerobic exercise effects on neuroelectric and behavioral measures of action monitoring , 2006, Neuroscience.

[2]  Arthur F. Kramer,et al.  Assessing the development of automatic processing: An application of dual-task and event-related brain potential methodologies , 1988, Biological Psychology.

[3]  Charles H Hillman,et al.  The relation of aerobic fitness to neuroelectric indices of cognitive and motor task preparation. , 2010, Psychophysiology.

[4]  A. Diamond The Early Development of Executive Functions. , 2006 .

[5]  N. Meiran Modeling cognitive control in task-switching , 2000, Psychological research.

[6]  T W Picton,et al.  The effects of age on human event-related potentials. , 1984, Psychophysiology.

[7]  A. Kramer,et al.  Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[8]  E. Donchin,et al.  Performance of concurrent tasks: a psychophysiological analysis of the reciprocity of information-processing resources. , 1983, Science.

[9]  J. Polich,et al.  Meta-analysis of P300 normative aging studies. , 1996, Psychophysiology.

[10]  Frank Rösler,et al.  Is task switching nothing but cue priming? Evidence from ERPs , 2008, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[11]  S. Monsell,et al.  Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .

[12]  A. Beck,et al.  An inventory for measuring depression. , 1961, Archives of general psychiatry.

[13]  J. Polich Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[14]  E Donchin,et al.  A metric for thought: a comparison of P300 latency and reaction time. , 1981, Science.

[15]  A. Kok On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[16]  Ritske de Jong,et al.  Strategical determinants of compatibility effects with task uncertainty , 1995 .

[17]  J. Polich,et al.  Task difficulty, probability, and inter-stimulus interval as determinants of P300 from auditory stimuli. , 1987, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[18]  F. Craik,et al.  Lifespan cognition: Mechanisms of change. , 2006 .

[19]  Natalie A. Phillips,et al.  Behavioural and electrophysiological measures of task switching during single and mixed-task conditions , 2006, Biological Psychology.

[20]  Matthew B. Pontifex,et al.  Fitness and action monitoring: Evidence for improved cognitive flexibility in young adults , 2008, Neuroscience.

[21]  P. Ekkekakis,et al.  Psychobiology of Physical Activity , 2006, Sport & Exercise Psychology Review.

[22]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  Task-set reconfiguration with predictable and unpredictable task switches , 2003, Memory & cognition.

[23]  M. Hagströmer,et al.  The International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ): a study of concurrent and construct validity , 2006, Public Health Nutrition.

[24]  Arthur F. Kramer,et al.  Physical Activity and Executive Control: Implications for Increased Cognitive Health during Older Adulthood , 2004, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[25]  Margot J. Taylor,et al.  Guidelines for using human event-related potentials to study cognition: recording standards and publication criteria. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[26]  E. Donchin Presidential address, 1980. Surprise!...Surprise? , 1981, Psychophysiology.

[27]  R. Emmerson,et al.  Age and fitness effects on EEG, ERPs, visual sensitivity, and cognition , 1990, Neurobiology of Aging.

[28]  B. Ainsworth,et al.  International physical activity questionnaire: 12-country reliability and validity. , 2003, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.

[29]  Charles H Hillman,et al.  A cross-sectional examination of age and physical activity on performance and event-related brain potentials in a task switching paradigm. , 2006, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[30]  A. Kramer,et al.  Fitness Effects on the Cognitive Function of Older Adults , 2003, Psychological science.

[31]  Yuji Takeda,et al.  General physical activity levels influence positive and negative priming effects in young adults , 2009, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[32]  C. Dolisi,et al.  P300, N400, aerobic fitness, and maximal aerobic exercise. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[33]  Charles H. Hillman,et al.  Age and physical activity influences on action monitoring during task switching , 2006, Neurobiology of Aging.

[34]  Charles H Hillman,et al.  The relationship of age and cardiovascular fitness to cognitive and motor processes. , 2002, Psychophysiology.

[35]  S. Nieuwenhuis,et al.  Mental fatigue and task control: planning and preparation. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[36]  J. Scisco,et al.  Cardiovascular fitness and executive control during task-switching: an ERP study. , 2008, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[37]  U. Lindenberger,et al.  Adult age differences in task switching. , 2000, Psychology and aging.