Covert Orienting of Attention in the Rat and the Role of Striatal Dopamine

Attention can be directed to a location in the absence of overt signs of orienting, a phenomenon termed “covert orienting.” The ability to orient attention covertly has been well documented in humans, but recent progress has been made with the operational definition of the processes involved in covert orienting. Reaction times to visual targets are quickened when attention is drawn to the location of the subsequent target, and processes such as disengagement, maintenance, and movement of attention can be dissociated by using this method. The possible involvement of striatal dopamine in covert orienting is disputed, with conflicting reports of deficits in covert orienting in patients with Parkinson’s disease. To examine the significance of dopamine in the striatum in attentional processes, a test of covert orienting, analogous to that used in humans, was devised for the rat. Unilateral dopamine-depleting lesions of the striatum resulted in increases in mean reaction times contralateral to the side of the lesion, but reaction times did not change differentially as a function of the requirements to maintain, disengage, or shift attention. These findings add additional support to the hypothesis that the deficit that appears as hemineglect observed after striatal damage reflects a motor impairment rather than damage in neural systems underlying mechanisms for directing attention.

[1]  G. Breese,et al.  Depletion of brain noradrenaline and dopamine by 6‐hydroxydopamine , 1971, British journal of pharmacology.

[2]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Temporal and spatial characteristics of selective encoding from visual displays , 1972 .

[3]  P. Teitelbaum,et al.  Further analysis of sensory inattention following lateral hypothalamic damage in rats. , 1974, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[4]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[5]  Y. Agid,et al.  Micro topography of Tyrosine Hydroxylase, Glutamic Acid Decarboxylase, and Choline Acetyltransferase in the Substantia Nigra and Ventral Tegmental Area of Control and Parkinsonian Brains , 1981, Journal of neurochemistry.

[6]  J. Jonides Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind's eye's movement , 1981 .

[7]  M. Posner,et al.  Neural systems control of spatial orienting. , 1982, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[8]  F. J. Friedrich,et al.  COGNITION AND THE BASAL GANGLIA , 1984 .

[9]  F. J. Friedrich,et al.  Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[10]  J. Jonides,et al.  On the Cost and Benefit of Cost and Benefit , 1984 .

[11]  M. Posner,et al.  How do the parietal lobes direct covert attention? , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  M. Posner,et al.  Deficits in human visual spatial attention following thalamic lesions. , 1987, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[13]  S. Petersen,et al.  Contributions of the pulvinar to visual spatial attention , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of visual attention in progressive supranuclear palsy. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[15]  P. J. Green,et al.  Density Estimation for Statistics and Data Analysis , 1987 .

[16]  T. Nagatsu [Biochemistry of Parkinson's disease]. , 1988, Seikagaku. The Journal of Japanese Biochemical Society.

[17]  T. Robbins,et al.  Elementary processes of response selection mediated by distinct regions of the striatum , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[18]  G. Geffen,et al.  Catecholamines and the covert orientation of attention in humans , 1989, Neuropsychologia.

[19]  T. Robbins,et al.  Effects of unilateral dorsal and ventral striatal dopamine depletion on visual neglect in the rat: A neural and behavioural analysis , 1989, Neuroscience.

[20]  G. Geffen,et al.  Covert orientation of visual attention in Parkinson's disease: An impairment in the maintenance of attention , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  T. Robbins,et al.  The Role of the Striatum in the Mental Chronometry of Action: A Theoretical Review , 1990, Reviews in the neurosciences.

[22]  K. Hirayama,et al.  Covert orienting attention in Parkinson's disease. , 1990, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[23]  G. Mckhann,et al.  Diseases of the Nervous System: Clinical Neurobiology , 1992 .

[24]  Jon Driver,et al.  The neurobiology of selective attention , 1992, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[25]  D. Robinson,et al.  Covert orienting of attention in macaques. I. Effects of behavioral context. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  F. J. Friedrich,et al.  Behavioral and computational models of spatial attention. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[27]  D. Robinson,et al.  Dopamine dependent reaction time deficits in patients with parkinson's disease are task specific , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  G. Geffen,et al.  Event-related potentials associated with covert orientation of visual attention in Parkinson's disease , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Re-orientation of attention in Parkinson's disease: An extension to the vibrotactile modality , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[30]  D L Robinson,et al.  Covert orienting of attention in macaques. III. Contributions of the superior colliculus. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  U. Castiello,et al.  Covert visuospatial attentional mechanisms in Parkinson's disease. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[32]  P. Bushnell Overt orienting in the rat: parametric studies of cued detection of visual targets. , 1995, Behavioral neuroscience.