Relationship of Head Circumference to Measures of School Performance

To study the possible relationship of head circumference (HC) to learning problems in children, a retrospective study was done, using records from 360 subjects who had been evaluated between the years 1976 and 1981. The data selected from the records included standardized academic test results, e.g., IQ scores and school achievement test results, as well as determinations of visual motor abilities. Statistical analyses demonstrated a positive correlation between full scale IQ (FSIQ) and HC (p < 0.005). The incidence of specific learning disabilities based on significant academic and achievement discrepancies was 54% in those with HC > 2 S.D., 39% for normocephalic, and 23% for those with HC < 2 S.D. Comparison of HC with results from the Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT) demonstrated a tendency for those children with HC > 2 S.D. to have lower arithmetic scores when compared to results of children with normocephaly. Scores for spelling and reading ability did not demonstrate this tendency. The results of this study were found to be somewhat incongruous to those of previous studies of similar children. Further studies using large unselected populations are needed to better define the risks to learning that may be related to the extremes of head circumference.

[1]  J. Snow,et al.  Subgroups based on medical, developmental, and growth variables with a sample of children and adolescents referred for learning difficulties , 1989 .

[2]  J. Hewitt,et al.  Macrocephaly and Minor Congenital Anomalies in Children with Learning Problems , 1984, Journal of developmental and behavioral pediatrics : JDBP.

[3]  Richard D. Smith,et al.  Abnormal Head Circumference in Learning‐disabled Children , 1981, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[4]  NORMAL PRESSURE HYDROCEPHALUS AND ARRESTED HYDROCEPHALUS , 1977, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[5]  J. Rapoport,et al.  Minor physical anomalies in normal, neurotic, learning disabled, and severely disturbed children , 1975, Journal of autism and childhood schizophrenia.

[6]  W. Mcalister,et al.  Intelligence, reading achievement, physical size and social class. A study of St. Louis Caucasian boys aged 8-0 to 9-6 years, attending regular schools. , 1974, The Journal of pediatrics.

[7]  W. Demyer Megalencephaly in children , 1972, Neurology.

[8]  G. Avery,et al.  The clinical significance of "measurement microcephaly". , 1972, American journal of diseases of children.

[9]  K. Nelson,et al.  Head Size at One Year as a Predictor of Four‐Year IQ , 1970, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[10]  H. P. Martin Microcephaly and mental retardation. , 1970, American journal of diseases of children.

[11]  H. B. Pryor,et al.  Abnormally small head size and intellect in children. , 1968, The Journal of pediatrics.

[12]  G. Nellhaus Head circumference from birth to eighteen years. Practical composite international and interracial graphs. , 1968, Pediatrics.

[13]  R. Feldt,et al.  HEAD CIRCUMFERENCE, MENTAL RETARDATION, AND GROWTH FAILURE. , 1965, Pediatrics.

[14]  R. G. Hunt,et al.  Social Class and Mental Illness , 1959 .

[15]  A. B. Hollingshead,et al.  Social Class and Mental Illness , 1964 .

[16]  Fredrick C. Redlich,et al.  Social class and mental illness: Community study. , 1958 .