Individual differences in non-verbal number acuity correlate with maths achievement

Human mathematical competence emerges from two representational systems. Competence in some domains of mathematics, such as calculus, relies on symbolic representations that are unique to humans who have undergone explicit teaching. More basic numerical intuitions are supported by an evolutionarily ancient approximate number system that is shared by adults, infants and non-human animals—these groups can all represent the approximate number of items in visual or auditory arrays without verbally counting, and use this capacity to guide everyday behaviour such as foraging. Despite the widespread nature of the approximate number system both across species and across development, it is not known whether some individuals have a more precise non-verbal ‘number sense’ than others. Furthermore, the extent to which this system interfaces with the formal, symbolic maths abilities that humans acquire by explicit instruction remains unknown. Here we show that there are large individual differences in the non-verbal approximation abilities of 14-year-old children, and that these individual differences in the present correlate with children’s past scores on standardized maths achievement tests, extending all the way back to kindergarten. Moreover, this correlation remains significant when controlling for individual differences in other cognitive and performance factors. Our results show that individual differences in achievement in school mathematics are related to individual differences in the acuity of an evolutionarily ancient, unlearned approximate number sense. Further research will determine whether early differences in number sense acuity affect later maths learning, whether maths education enhances number sense acuity, and the extent to which tertiary factors can affect both.

[1]  R. Church,et al.  A mode control model of counting and timing processes. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[2]  S. Dehaene,et al.  An open trial assessment of "The Number Race", an adaptive computer game for remediation of dyscalculia , 2006, Behavioral and Brain Functions.

[3]  E. Spelke,et al.  Language and Conceptual Development series Core systems of number , 2004 .

[4]  Jamie I. D. Campbell Handbook of mathematical cognition , 2004 .

[5]  T. Senn,et al.  The Contribution of Executive Functions to Emergent Mathematic Skills in Preschool Children , 2004, Developmental neuropsychology.

[6]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Abstract representations of numbers in the animal and human brain , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.

[7]  Rochel Gelman,et al.  Variability signatures distinguish verbal from nonverbal counting for both large and small numbers , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[8]  G. Mandler,et al.  Subitizing: an analysis of its component processes. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[9]  M. Denckla,et al.  Rapid ‘automatized’ naming (R.A.N.): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities , 1976, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  C R Gallistel,et al.  Numerical Subtraction in the Pigeon: Evidence for a Linear Subjective Number Scale , 2001, Psychological science.

[11]  M. Mazzocco,et al.  A longitudinal analysis of sex differences in math and spatial skills in primary school age children. , 2006, Learning and individual differences.

[12]  Andreas Nieder,et al.  A parieto-frontal network for visual numerical information in the monkey. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[13]  E. Spelke,et al.  The construction of large number representations in adults , 2003, Cognition.

[14]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Developmental change in the acuity of the "Number Sense": The Approximate Number System in 3-, 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds and adults. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[15]  M. Mazzocco,et al.  A Longitudinal Assessment of Executive Function Skills and Their Association with Math Performance , 2007, Child neuropsychology : a journal on normal and abnormal development in childhood and adolescence.

[16]  H S Terrace,et al.  Ordering of the numerosities 1 to 9 by monkeys. , 1998, Science.

[17]  C. Gallistel The organization of learning , 1990 .

[18]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Principles underlying the design of "The Number Race", an adaptive computer game for remediation of dyscalculia , 2006, Behavioral and brain functions : BBF.

[19]  E. Spelke,et al.  Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants , 2000, Cognition.

[20]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics. , 1998 .

[21]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Exact and Approximate Arithmetic in an Amazonian Indigene Group , 2004, Science.

[22]  S. Carey Bootstrapping & the origin of concepts , 2004, Daedalus.

[23]  P. Gordon Numerical Cognition Without Words: Evidence from Amazonia , 2004, Science.

[24]  Michèle M. M. Mazzocco,et al.  Complexities in identifying and defining mathematics learning disability in the primary school-age years , 2003, Annals of dyslexia.

[25]  L. A. Kurdek,et al.  Predicting reading and mathematics achievement in fourth-grade children from kindergarten readiness scores. , 2001 .