The ecological validity of traditional memory evaluation in relation with controlled memory processes and routinization.

The neuropsychological evaluation of memory by traditional tests raises questions about their ecological validity, as the results on these tests often have little relation to the memory complaints. In an attempt to explain this lack of relationship, the present study had two objectives: (1) explore the ecological superiority of the Process Dissociation Procedure (PDP) over traditional memory tests and (2) explore the effects of routinization on the relationship between memory complaints and memory tests. Thirty-three participants aged 55-86 years were given the PDP (memory evaluation), two questionnaires evaluating daily memory complaints (QAM and CDS) and a questionnaire evaluating routinization (EPR). The results indicate that the PDP, with its measure of controlled processes, is more ecological than traditional memory tests for elderly people. As well, the participants' lifestyle (routinized versus non-routinized) influenced their results on memory tests. The results are discussed in relation to neuropsychological evaluation and rehabilitation.

[1]  Patrick E. Shrout,et al.  Toward a psychometric analysis of violations of the independence assumption in process dissociation , 1997 .

[2]  M. Isingrini,et al.  Does priming with awareness reflect explicit contamination? An approach with a response-time measure in word-stem completion , 2005, Consciousness and Cognition.

[3]  D L Hintzman,et al.  Violations of the independence assumption in process dissociation. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[4]  Y. Stern The Concept of Cognitive Reserve: A Catalyst for Research , 2003, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[5]  M. Storandt,et al.  Predictors of subjective memory in older adults. , 2004, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[6]  L L Jacoby,et al.  Invariance in automatic influences of memory: toward a user's guide for the process-dissociation procedure. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[7]  Larry L. Jacoby,et al.  Recollection and familiarity: Process-dissociation. , 2000 .

[8]  Sandra E. Trehub,et al.  Aging and cognitive processes , 1982 .

[9]  Yaakov Stern,et al.  Cognitive Reserve and Lifestyle , 2003, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[10]  A. Alpérovitch,et al.  Empirical evaluation of the ‘Cognitive Difficulties Scale’ for assessment of memory complaints in general practice: A study of 1628 cognitively normal subjects aged 45–75 years , 1993 .

[11]  Y. Stern,et al.  Influence of leisure activity on the incidence of Alzheimer’s Disease , 2001, Neurology.

[12]  M. J. Emerson,et al.  The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[13]  A P Yonelinas,et al.  Dissociating automatic and controlled processes in a memory-search task: Beyond implicit memory , 1995, Psychological research.

[14]  R. Hébert,et al.  Risk factors for Alzheimer's disease: a prospective analysis from the Canadian Study of Health and Aging. , 2002, American journal of epidemiology.

[15]  Jean-François Dartigues,et al.  Preferences for routines in older people: associations with cognitive and psychological vulnerability , 2006, International journal of geriatric psychiatry.

[16]  T. Meulemans,et al.  Evaluation et prise en charge des troubles mnésiques , 2003 .

[17]  Ken A Paller,et al.  Neural correlates of conceptual implicit memory and their contamination of putative neural correlates of explicit memory. , 2007, Learning & memory.

[18]  Fergus I. M. Craik,et al.  Aging and cognitive deficits : The role of attentional resources , 1982 .

[19]  A J Zautra,et al.  Analyzing the Trait of Routinization in Older Adults , 1991, International journal of aging & human development.

[20]  E. Reingold,et al.  Theory and measurement in the study of unconscious processes , 1990 .

[21]  Y. Stern What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept , 2002, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[22]  L. Light Memory and aging: four hypotheses in search of data. , 1991, Annual review of psychology.

[23]  D. B. Mitchell,et al.  The handbook of aging and cognition , 2001 .

[24]  F. Craik,et al.  The Oxford handbook of memory , 2006 .

[25]  Jean-François Dartigues,et al.  The Effect of Education on Cognitive Performances and Its Implication for the Constitution of the Cognitive Reserve , 2003, Developmental neuropsychology.

[26]  J. Jennings,et al.  Process dissociation procedure: Memory testing in populations with brain damage , 1996 .

[27]  Dianne C. Berry,et al.  Implicit memory: intention and awareness revisited , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[28]  Larry L. Jacoby,et al.  In defense of functional independence: Violations of assumptions underlying the process-dissociation procedure? , 1997 .

[29]  L. Jacoby A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory , 1991 .

[30]  R. Kastenbaum When Aging Begins , 1984, Research on aging.

[31]  L L Jacoby,et al.  Automatic versus intentional uses of memory: aging, attention, and control. , 1993, Psychology and aging.

[32]  B. Winblad,et al.  An active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life might protect against dementia , 2004, The Lancet Neurology.

[33]  Andrew P. Yonelinas,et al.  Separating conscious and unconscious influences of memory: measuring recollection , 1993 .

[34]  S. Hynie Assessment in Geriatric Psychopharmacology Edited by Thomas Crook, Steven Ferris and Raymond Bartus, Mark Powley, Connecticut, 1983. , 1986, Neuropharmacology.

[35]  J. R. Doyle Semantic activation without conscious identification in dichotic listening , parafoveal vision , and visual masking : A survey and appraisal , 2008 .

[36]  L L Jacoby,et al.  Unconscious perception: attention, awareness, and control. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[37]  J. Swendsen,et al.  Routinization and emotional well-being: an experience sampling investigation in an elderly French sample. , 2003, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[38]  F. Collette,et al.  Further exploration of controlled and automatic memory processes in early Alzheimer's disease. , 2005, Neuropsychology.

[39]  Andrew P. Yonelinas,et al.  The relation between conscious and unconscious (automatic) influences: A declaration of independence. , 1997 .

[40]  Arthur F Kramer,et al.  Achieving and maintaining cognitive vitality with aging. , 2002, Mayo Clinic proceedings.

[41]  A. Drewnowski,et al.  Journals of Gerontology , 2001 .

[42]  C. Schooler,et al.  The continuing effects of substantively complex work on the intellectual functioning of older workers. , 1999, Psychology and aging.

[43]  A. Parkin,et al.  Human memory , 1999, Current Biology.

[44]  S. Perelman,et al.  Occupational Activities and Cognitive Reserve: a Frontier Approach Applied to the Survey on Health, Ageing, and Retirement in Europe , 2006 .

[45]  Louise M. Reid,et al.  Subjective Memory Complaints and Cognitive Impairment in Older People , 2006, Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders.

[46]  T. Shallice Specific impairments of planning. , 1982, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[47]  J. Schneider,et al.  Participation in cognitively stimulating activities and risk of incident Alzheimer disease. , 2002, JAMA.

[48]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.

[49]  S. Riedel-Heller,et al.  Incident Dementia Cases and Mortality , 2006, Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders.

[50]  Laura Fratiglioni,et al.  Late-life engagement in social and leisure activities is associated with a decreased risk of dementia: a longitudinal study from the Kungsholmen project. , 2002, American journal of epidemiology.

[51]  L L Jacoby,et al.  Separating habit and recollection: memory slips, process dissociations, and probability matching. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[52]  L L Jacoby,et al.  Separating habit and recollection in young and older adults: effects of elaborative processing and distinctiveness. , 1999, Psychology and aging.

[53]  Stéphane Adam Nouvelles techniques d'évaluation de la mémoire : procédure de dissociation des processus et paradigme R/K , 2003 .

[54]  J. Bouisson Routinization preferences, anxiety, and depression in an elderly French sample , 2002 .

[55]  R. Bjork,et al.  Primary versus secondary rehearsal In an imagined voice: Differential effects on recognition memory and perceptual identification , 1988 .

[56]  E M Reingold,et al.  Toward a redefinition of implicit memory: process dissociations following elaborative processing and self-generation. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[57]  D. L. Hintzman,et al.  Consequences and causes of correlations in process dissociation. , 1997 .

[58]  L. Berkman,et al.  From social integration to health: Durkheim in the new millennium. , 2000, Social science & medicine.

[59]  D. B. Mitchell,et al.  Age differences in implicit memory: conceptual, perceptual, or methodological? , 2003, Psychology and aging.

[60]  L L Jacoby,et al.  An opposition procedure for detecting age-related deficits in recollection: telling effects of repetition. , 1997, Psychology and aging.

[61]  L L Jacoby,et al.  Ironic effects of repetition: measuring age-related differences in memory. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[62]  J. Nigg,et al.  Is ADHD a disinhibitory disorder? , 2001, Psychological bulletin.