Muscarinic antagonist enhances one-trial food-aversion learning in the mollusc Pleurobranchaea

One hour before training, in two replicate studies on the sea slug Pleurobranchaea californica, all animals (N = 114) received body-cavity injections of scopolamine, oxotremorine, or the equivalent volume of the saline/seawater vehicle that was used to inject the drugs. The low drug doses (2 mumol/kg) were near the threshold for generating observable neurophysiological responses, but did not affect feeding thresholds arising to a stimulus derived from beer (Sbr) and to one derived from squid (Ssq). Before training, the animals did not discriminate between Sbr and Ssq, as indicated by similar thresholds to both stimuli. During training, experimental animals in each injection group received Sbr alone for 10 sec and then paired with electric shocks for 50 sec; control animals received shocks 1 hr after Sbr. Postconditioning tests began 12 hr after training and were repeated once daily thereafter. After training, all experimental groups exhibited the expected aversive behavior to Sbr, as indicated by 100- to 1000-fold increases in feeding thresholds, and retained low thresholds to Ssq, but the scopolamine animals were better able to discriminate between Sbr and Ssq than either of the other experimental groups. The aversive responses to Sbr increased over a 3-day period, but there appeared to be no difference between injection groups on such a long-duration "consolidation" phase. Of the control groups, only the scopolamine animals exhibited low feeding thresholds to both Sbr and Ssq. The other control groups exhibited similar behavior as the experimental animals, indicating that associative factors relating to the 1-hr separation between Sbr and shocks may have produced the behavior in these control animals. Thus, by comparison to the other injections, scopolamine 1) increased the ability of the experimental animals to make the discrimination between Sbr and Ssq, and 2) prevented learning to avoid Sbr in the control animals. An accompanying paper provides a detailed characterization of muscarinic receptor pharmacology in Pleurobranchaea.

[1]  Y. Dudai,et al.  In vivo modulation of the number of muscarinic receptors in rat brain by cholinergic ligands. , 1981, European journal of pharmacology.

[2]  H. Yamamura,et al.  ALTERATIONS IN MUSCARINIC CHOLINERGIC RECEPTOR BINDING IN HUNTINGTON'S DISEASE , 1979 .

[3]  C. Cohan,et al.  Discriminative behavior and Pavlovian conditioning in the mollusc Pleurobranchaea. , 1986, Journal of neurobiology.

[4]  D L Price,et al.  Alzheimer's disease: a disorder of cortical cholinergic innervation. , 1983, Science.

[5]  J. Venter Evolution and structure of neurotransmitter receptors , 1984 .

[6]  E. Kandel,et al.  Molecular biology of learning: modulation of transmitter release. , 1982, Science.

[7]  G. Mpitsos,et al.  Stereoselective L-[3H]quinuclidinyl benzilate-binding sites in nervous tissue of Aplysia californica: evidence for muscarinic receptors , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[8]  G. Mpitsos,et al.  Evidence for heterogeneity of muscarinic receptors in the mollusc Pleurobranchaea , 1988, Brain Research Bulletin.

[9]  L R Squire,et al.  The pharmacology of memory: a neurobiological perspective. , 1981, Annual review of pharmacology and toxicology.

[10]  P. Salvaterra,et al.  [1251]2α‐BUNGAROTOXIN AND [3H]QUINUCLIDINYLBENZILATE BINDING IN CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEMS OF DIFFERENT SPECIES , 1979 .

[11]  J. Kehoe,et al.  The physiological role of three acetylcholine receptors in synaptic transmission in Aplysia , 1972, The Journal of physiology.

[12]  E. Marder,et al.  Identification and effects of neural transmitters in invertebrates. , 1976, Annual review of pharmacology and toxicology.

[13]  D. Drachman,et al.  Memory and cognitive function in man , 1977, Neurology.

[14]  G. S. Rosenberg,et al.  Cholinomimetic Agents and Human Memory: Preliminary Observations in Alzheimer’s Disease , 1981 .

[15]  J. Byrne Cellular analysis of associative learning. , 1987, Physiological reviews.

[16]  W. Davis,et al.  Learning: Classical and Avoidance Conditioning in the Mollusk Pleurobranchaea , 1973, Science.

[17]  J. Venter,et al.  Monoclonal antibodies detect the conservation of muscarinic cholinergic receptor structure from Drosophila to human brain and detect possible structural homology with alpha 1-adrenergic receptors. , 1984, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[18]  R. Gillette,et al.  Mechanism for food avoidance learning in the central pattern generator of feeding behavior of Pleurobranchae californica. , 1986, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[19]  E. Kandel,et al.  Classical conditioning in a simple withdrawal reflex in Aplysia californica , 1981, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[20]  E. Kandel,et al.  Classical conditioning in Aplysia californica. , 1979, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[21]  T. Carew,et al.  Invertebrate learning and memory: from behavior to molecules. , 1986, Annual review of neuroscience.

[22]  George J. Mpitsos,et al.  Learning in Gastropod Molluscs , 1985 .

[23]  C S Cohan,et al.  Comparison of differential Pavlovian conditioning in whole animals and physiological preparations of Pleurobranchaea: implications of motor pattern variability. , 1986, Journal of neurobiology.

[24]  C S Cohan,et al.  Differential Pavlovian conditioning in the mollusc Pleurobranchaea. , 1986, Journal of neurobiology.

[25]  A. Gelperin Rapid food-aversion learning by a terrestrial mollusk. , 1975, Science.

[26]  W. Davis,et al.  Neural correlate of behavioral plasticity in command neurons of Pleurobranchaea. , 1978, Science.

[27]  R. Russell Cholinergic system in behavior: the search for mechanisms of action. , 1982, Annual review of pharmacology and toxicology.

[28]  D. Drachman,et al.  Human memory and the cholinergic system. A relationship to aging? , 1974, Archives of neurology.

[29]  E. Kandel,et al.  Associative Learning in Aplysia: evidence for conditioned fear in an invertebrate. , 1981, Science.

[30]  B. J. Winer Statistical Principles in Experimental Design , 1992 .

[31]  A. D. McClellan,et al.  Learning: a model system for physiological studies. , 1978, Science.

[32]  A. C. Collins,et al.  Chronic scopolamine treatment and brain cholinergic function , 1984, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[33]  J A Deutsch,et al.  The Cholinergic Synapse and the Site of Memory , 1971, Science.

[34]  D. Alkon Calcium-mediated reduction of ionic currents: a biophysical memory trace. , 1984, Science.