Distributional reaction time properties in the Eriksen task: marked differences or hidden similarities with the Simon task?
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] K. Richard Ridderinkhof,et al. Dopamine Agonists and the Suppression of Impulsive Motor Actions in Parkinson Disease , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[2] Wolf Schwarz,et al. Response time models of delta plots with negative-going slopes , 2012, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.
[3] R. Ratcliff,et al. Diffusion models of the flanker task: Discrete versus gradual attentional selection , 2011, Cognitive Psychology.
[4] Gabriela Garrido Rodriguez,et al. Attentional Inhibition in Bilingual Naming Performance: Evidence from Delta-Plot Analyses , 2011, Front. Psychology.
[5] James D. Miles,et al. Reaction time distribution analysis of spatial correspondence effects , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[6] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. Version unknown SOURCE ( OR PART OF THE FOLLOWING SOURCE ) : Type article Title To head or to heed ? Beyond the surface of selective action inhibition : a review , 2022 .
[7] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. Subthalamic nucleus stimulation influences expression and suppression of impulsive behaviour in Parkinson's disease. , 2010, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[8] Jeffrey N Rouder,et al. Exploring the differences in distributional properties between Stroop and Simon effects using delta plots , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[9] M. Steinhauser,et al. A dual-stage two-phase model of selective attention. , 2010, Psychological review.
[10] Borís Burle,et al. Error Negativity Does Not Reflect Conflict: A Reappraisal of Conflict Monitoring and Anterior Cingulate Cortex Activity , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[11] Richard D. Morey,et al. Delta Plots and Coherent Distribution Ordering , 2008 .
[12] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. Inefficient response inhibition in individuals with mild cognitive impairment , 2007, Neuropsychologia.
[13] Scott D. Brown,et al. On the linear relation between the mean and the standard deviation of a response time distribution. , 2007, Psychological review.
[14] B. Burle,et al. Sequential compatibility effects and cognitive control: does conflict really matter? , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[15] Edmund Wascher,et al. Dynamic aspects of stimulus-response correspondence: evidence for two mechanisms involved in the Simon effect. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[16] Peter C. M. Molenaar,et al. On the relation between the mean and the variance of a diffusion model response time distribution , 2005 .
[17] K Richard Ridderinkhof,et al. Delta plots in the study of individual differences: new tools reveal response inhibition deficits in AD/Hd that are eliminated by methylphenidate treatment. , 2005, Journal of abnormal psychology.
[18] Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al. A note on the sampling properties of the Vincentizing (quantile averaging) procedure , 2004 .
[19] J. Danckert. Common Mechanisms in Perception and Action: Attention and Performance XIX Wolfgang Prinz, Bernhard Hommel (Eds.), Oxford University Press, 2002, Price: £ 65.00, ISBN: 0-19-851069 , 2003, Neuropsychologia.
[20] Richard Ridderinkhof. Micro- and macro-adjustments of task set: activation and suppression in conflict tasks , 2002, Psychological research.
[21] Borís Burle,et al. Executive control in the Simon effect: an electromyographic and distributional analysis , 2002, Psychological research.
[22] Rolf Verleger,et al. Validity and boundary conditions of automatic response activation in the Simon task. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[23] F. Vidal,et al. Effect of the irrelevant location of the response signal on choice reaction time: an electromyographic study in humans. , 1999, Psychophysiology.
[24] E. Lauber,et al. Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[25] D. Meyer,et al. A Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1993 .
[26] M. M. van den Berg-Lenssen,et al. Detection of EMG onset in ERP research. , 1993, Psychophysiology.
[27] E. Donchin,et al. Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[28] G Mulder,et al. Selective response activation can begin before stimulus recognition is complete: a psychophysiological and error analysis of continuous flow. , 1990, Acta psychologica.
[29] A. Osman,et al. Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.
[30] C. Eriksen,et al. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , 2004 .
[31] R. Ratcliff. Group reaction time distributions and an analysis of distribution statistics. , 1979, Psychological bulletin.
[32] J. R. Simon,et al. Reactions toward the source of stimulation. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.
[33] D. A. Grant. Analysis-of-variance tests in the analysis and comparison of curves. , 1956, Psychological bulletin.
[34] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. Activation and suppression in conflict tasks: Empirical clarification through distributional analyses. , 2002 .
[35] Werner Wolf,et al. Onset Detection in Surface Electromyographic Signals: A Systematic Comparison of Methods , 2001, EURASIP J. Adv. Signal Process..
[36] J. Ridley. Studies of Interference in Serial Verbal Reactions , 2001 .
[37] C. Eriksen,et al. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .
[38] B. J. Winer. Design and analysis of factorial experiments. , 1962 .
[39] S. B. Vincent. The function of the vibrissae in the behavior of the white rat , 1912 .