Distributional reaction time properties in the Eriksen task: marked differences or hidden similarities with the Simon task?

[1]  K. Richard Ridderinkhof,et al.  Dopamine Agonists and the Suppression of Impulsive Motor Actions in Parkinson Disease , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[2]  Wolf Schwarz,et al.  Response time models of delta plots with negative-going slopes , 2012, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[3]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Diffusion models of the flanker task: Discrete versus gradual attentional selection , 2011, Cognitive Psychology.

[4]  Gabriela Garrido Rodriguez,et al.  Attentional Inhibition in Bilingual Naming Performance: Evidence from Delta-Plot Analyses , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[5]  James D. Miles,et al.  Reaction time distribution analysis of spatial correspondence effects , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[6]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Version unknown SOURCE ( OR PART OF THE FOLLOWING SOURCE ) : Type article Title To head or to heed ? Beyond the surface of selective action inhibition : a review , 2022 .

[7]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Subthalamic nucleus stimulation influences expression and suppression of impulsive behaviour in Parkinson's disease. , 2010, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[8]  Jeffrey N Rouder,et al.  Exploring the differences in distributional properties between Stroop and Simon effects using delta plots , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[9]  M. Steinhauser,et al.  A dual-stage two-phase model of selective attention. , 2010, Psychological review.

[10]  Borís Burle,et al.  Error Negativity Does Not Reflect Conflict: A Reappraisal of Conflict Monitoring and Anterior Cingulate Cortex Activity , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[11]  Richard D. Morey,et al.  Delta Plots and Coherent Distribution Ordering , 2008 .

[12]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Inefficient response inhibition in individuals with mild cognitive impairment , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  On the linear relation between the mean and the standard deviation of a response time distribution. , 2007, Psychological review.

[14]  B. Burle,et al.  Sequential compatibility effects and cognitive control: does conflict really matter? , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  Edmund Wascher,et al.  Dynamic aspects of stimulus-response correspondence: evidence for two mechanisms involved in the Simon effect. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  Peter C. M. Molenaar,et al.  On the relation between the mean and the variance of a diffusion model response time distribution , 2005 .

[17]  K Richard Ridderinkhof,et al.  Delta plots in the study of individual differences: new tools reveal response inhibition deficits in AD/Hd that are eliminated by methylphenidate treatment. , 2005, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[18]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  A note on the sampling properties of the Vincentizing (quantile averaging) procedure , 2004 .

[19]  J. Danckert Common Mechanisms in Perception and Action: Attention and Performance XIX Wolfgang Prinz, Bernhard Hommel (Eds.), Oxford University Press, 2002, Price: £ 65.00, ISBN: 0-19-851069 , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  Richard Ridderinkhof Micro- and macro-adjustments of task set: activation and suppression in conflict tasks , 2002, Psychological research.

[21]  Borís Burle,et al.  Executive control in the Simon effect: an electromyographic and distributional analysis , 2002, Psychological research.

[22]  Rolf Verleger,et al.  Validity and boundary conditions of automatic response activation in the Simon task. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[23]  F. Vidal,et al.  Effect of the irrelevant location of the response signal on choice reaction time: an electromyographic study in humans. , 1999, Psychophysiology.

[24]  E. Lauber,et al.  Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  D. Meyer,et al.  A Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation , 1993 .

[26]  M. M. van den Berg-Lenssen,et al.  Detection of EMG onset in ERP research. , 1993, Psychophysiology.

[27]  E. Donchin,et al.  Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[28]  G Mulder,et al.  Selective response activation can begin before stimulus recognition is complete: a psychophysiological and error analysis of continuous flow. , 1990, Acta psychologica.

[29]  A. Osman,et al.  Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.

[30]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , 2004 .

[31]  R. Ratcliff Group reaction time distributions and an analysis of distribution statistics. , 1979, Psychological bulletin.

[32]  J. R. Simon,et al.  Reactions toward the source of stimulation. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.

[33]  D. A. Grant Analysis-of-variance tests in the analysis and comparison of curves. , 1956, Psychological bulletin.

[34]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Activation and suppression in conflict tasks: Empirical clarification through distributional analyses. , 2002 .

[35]  Werner Wolf,et al.  Onset Detection in Surface Electromyographic Signals: A Systematic Comparison of Methods , 2001, EURASIP J. Adv. Signal Process..

[36]  J. Ridley Studies of Interference in Serial Verbal Reactions , 2001 .

[37]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[38]  B. J. Winer Design and analysis of factorial experiments. , 1962 .

[39]  S. B. Vincent The function of the vibrissae in the behavior of the white rat , 1912 .