Effects of prelinguistic milieu teaching and parent responsivity education on dyads involving children with intellectual disabilities.

This study tested the effect of a method of facilitating prelinguistic communication on parents' responsivity and children's communication and productive language development. The method involved Responsivity education for the parents and Prelinguistic Milieu Teaching for the children (RPMT). Thirty-nine prelinguistic toddlers with intellectual disabilities and their primary caregivers participated in this study. Parent-child pairs were randomly assigned to either the RPMT group or a control group. Communication and language were assessed at study entry and 6, 9, and 12 months later. RPMT facilitated parental responsivity in the posttreatment period. The effect of RPMT on growth rate of child-initiated comments (i.e., the most common type of initiating joint attention) varied by pretreatment measures of that variable. The effect of RPMT on growth rate of child-initiated requests (i.e., the most common type of initiating behavior regulation) varied by presence or absence of Down syndrome. Finally, the effect of RPMT on growth of productive language varied by pretreatment frequency of canonical vocal communication. Recommended alterations in PMT and implications for defining which nonspeaking children are appropriate for prelinguistic goals and treatment were discussed.

[1]  L. Menn,et al.  Phonological development : models, research, implications , 1994 .

[2]  P. Mundy,et al.  A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children , 1990, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[3]  Joop J. Hox,et al.  Applied Multilevel Analysis. , 1995 .

[4]  R. Thatcher Functional neuroimaging : technical foundations , 1994 .

[5]  P. Mundy Joint attention and social-emotional approach behavior in children with autism , 1995, Development and Psychopathology.

[6]  S. von Tetzchner,et al.  Communicative, sensorimotor, and language skills of young children with Down syndrome. , 1986, American journal of mental deficiency.

[7]  P. Mundy,et al.  Nonverbal communication skills in Down syndrome children. , 1988, Child development.

[8]  Elizabeth Bates,et al.  Books Recieved , 2022, CL.

[9]  Francis D. Horowitz Review of Child Development Research , 1975 .

[10]  Judith D. Singer,et al.  Using SAS PROC MIXED to Fit Multilevel Models, Hierarchical Models, and Individual Growth Models , 1998 .

[11]  S. Warren,et al.  Facilitating Self-Initiated Proto-Declaratives and Proto-Imperatives in Prelinguistic Children with Developmental Disabilities , 1999 .

[12]  S. Warren,et al.  Representational Ability as a Predictor of Later Expressive Vocabulary , 1999 .

[13]  G. Whitehurst,et al.  The continuity of babble and speech in children with specific expressive language delay. , 1991, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[14]  S. Warren,et al.  Prelinguistic Pragmatic Functions as Predictors of Later Expressive Vocabulary , 1999 .

[15]  M. Vihman,et al.  Individual differences in phonological development: ages one and three years. , 1987, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[16]  Anne H. Soukhanov,et al.  The american heritage dictionary of the english language , 1992 .

[17]  L. Leavitt,et al.  Improving the communication of people with Down syndrome , 1999 .

[18]  Eva Nick,et al.  The dependability of behavioral measurements: theory of generalizability for scores and profiles , 1973 .

[19]  Gregory Camilli,et al.  Application of a Method of Estimating DIF for Polytomous Test Items , 1999 .

[20]  P. Mundy,et al.  Nonverbal communication and early language acquisition in children with Down syndrome and in normally developing children. , 1995, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[21]  S. Warren,et al.  Important Distinctions in Measuring Maternal Responses to Communication in Prelinguistic Children with Disabilities , 2001 .

[22]  Yuk Fai Cheong,et al.  HLM 6: Hierarchical Linear and Nonlinear Modeling , 2000 .

[23]  B. Hart The Initial Growth of Expressive Vocabulary Among Children With Down Syndrome , 1996 .

[24]  S. Warren,et al.  Transitions in prelinguistic communication , 1998 .

[25]  S. Warren,et al.  Maternal responsivity predicts the prelinguistic communication intervention that facilitates generalized intentional communication. , 1998, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[26]  R S Chapman,et al.  Behavioral phenotype of individuals with Down syndrome. , 2000, Mental retardation and developmental disabilities research reviews.

[27]  J. Goodman,et al.  Played Out? Passive Behavior by Children With Down Syndrome During Unstructured Play , 2000 .

[28]  R. Sevcik,et al.  Breaking the speech barrier : language development through augmented means , 1996 .

[29]  S. Warren,et al.  Determining Spoken Language Prognosis in Children With Developmental Disabilities , 1998 .

[30]  D. Kimbrough Oiler,et al.  Metaphonology and Infant Vocalizations , 1986 .

[31]  L. Bloom The Transition from Infancy to Language: FROM 9 MONTHS TO 2 YEARS , 1993 .

[32]  Robin S. Chapman Processes in Language Acquisition and Disorders , 1992 .

[33]  Gillian Stevens,et al.  Socioeconomic indexes and the new 1980 census occupational classification scheme , 1985 .

[34]  Great Britain. Foreign Office.,et al.  Classification of occupations , 1960 .

[35]  S F Warren,et al.  Intentional communication elicits language-facilitating maternal responses in dyads with children who have developmental disabilities. , 2001, American journal of mental retardation : AJMR.

[36]  B. Richards Type/Token Ratios: what do they really tell us? , 1987, Journal of Child Language.

[37]  T. Humphries,et al.  A Basic Course in American Sign Language , 1981 .

[38]  P. Hanks,et al.  Collins dictionary of the English language , 1979 .

[39]  A. Kaiser,et al.  Enhancing Children's Communication: Research Foundations for Intervention , 1993 .

[40]  D. Thal,et al.  Language and gesture in late talkers: a 1-year follow-up. , 1991, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[41]  M. Cuskelly,et al.  Down Syndrome Temperament: The Stereotype at Middle Childhood and Adolescence , 1991 .

[42]  Donald B. Rubin,et al.  The Dependability of Behavioral Measurements: Theory of Generalizability for Scores and Profiles. , 1974 .

[43]  B. Robinson,et al.  Extrapolated raw scores for the second edition of the Bayley Scales of Infant Development. , 1996, American journal of mental retardation : AJMR.

[44]  S. Warren,et al.  Maternal Responsivity Mediates the Relationship Between Prelinguistic Intentional Communication and Later Language , 1999 .

[45]  S F Warren,et al.  Relative treatment effects of two prelinguistic communication interventions on language development in toddlers with developmental delays vary by maternal characteristics. , 2001, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[46]  Lois Bloom,et al.  Language and Interaction. (Book Reviews: The Transition from Infancy to Language. Acquiring the Power of Expression.) , 1995 .

[47]  M. Wilcox,et al.  Predicting language outcomes for young prelinguistic children with developmental delay. , 2000, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.