The Good, the Bad, and the Just: Justice Sensitivity Predicts Neural Response during Moral Evaluation of Actions Performed by Others

Morality is a fundamental component of human cultures and has been defined as prescriptive norms regarding how people should treat one another, including concepts such as justice, fairness, and rights. Using fMRI, the current study examined the extent to which dispositions in justice sensitivity (i.e., how individuals react to experiences of injustice and unfairness) predict behavioral ratings of praise and blame and how they modulate the online neural response and functional connectivity when participants evaluate morally laden (good and bad) everyday actions. Justice sensitivity did not impact the neuro-hemodynamic response in the action-observation network but instead influenced higher-order computational nodes in the right temporoparietal junction (rTPJ), right dorsolateral and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (rdlPFC, dmPFC) that process mental states understanding and maintain goal representations. Activity in these regions predicted praise and blame ratings. Further, the hemodynamic response in rTPJ showed a differentiation between good and bad actions 2 s before the response in rdlPFC. Evaluation of good actions was specifically associated with enhanced activity in dorsal striatum and increased the functional coupling between the rTPJ and the anterior cingulate cortex. Together, this study provides important knowledge in how individual differences in justice sensitivity impact neural computations that support psychological processes involved in moral judgment and mental-state reasoning.

[1]  B. Malle,et al.  Are intentionality judgments fundamentally moral , 2012 .

[2]  R. Saxe,et al.  Innocent intentions: A correlation between forgiveness for accidental harm and neural activity , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  Rebecca Saxe,et al.  The neural basis of belief encoding and integration in moral judgment , 2008, NeuroImage.

[4]  B. Balleine,et al.  The Role of the Dorsal Striatum in Reward and Decision-Making , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  Andrew D. Engell,et al.  The Neural Bases of Cognitive Conflict and Control in Moral Judgment , 2004, Neuron.

[6]  R. Saxe,et al.  The neural basis of the interaction between theory of mind and moral judgment , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[7]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Error Awareness and Salience Processing in the Oddball Task: Shared Neural Mechanisms , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[8]  M. Kronbichler,et al.  Thinking of mental and other representations: The roles of left and right temporo-parietal junction , 2006, Social neuroscience.

[9]  Mark D'Esposito,et al.  The salience network causally influences default mode network activity during moral reasoning. , 2013, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[10]  Scott T. Grafton Embodied Cognition and the Simulation of Action to Understand Others , 2009, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[11]  R. Marois,et al.  The roots of modern justice: cognitive and neural foundations of social norms and their enforcement , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.

[12]  R. Corcoran,et al.  The QCAE: A Questionnaire of Cognitive and Affective Empathy , 2009, European Psychiatry.

[13]  Rebecca Saxe,et al.  An fMRI Investigation of Spontaneous Mental State Inference for Moral Judgment , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[14]  Jean Decety,et al.  The speed of morality: a high-density electrical neuroimaging study. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  K. Vogeley,et al.  Parsing the neural correlates of moral cognition: ALE meta-analysis on morality, theory of mind, and empathy , 2012, Brain Structure and Function.

[16]  James Dungan,et al.  Where in the brain is morality? Everywhere and maybe nowhere , 2012, Social neuroscience.

[17]  F. Manes,et al.  The role of social cognition in moral judgment in frontotemporal dementia , 2011, Social neuroscience.

[18]  Michael Koenigs,et al.  Investigating emotion in moral cognition: a review of evidence from functional neuroimaging and neuropsychology. , 2007, British medical bulletin.

[19]  J. Decety,et al.  Imagining being the agent of actions that carry different moral consequences: An fMRI study , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  David H. Zald,et al.  The Neural Correlates of Third-Party Punishment , 2008, Neuron.

[21]  Katherine D. Kinzler,et al.  The contribution of emotion and cognition to moral sensitivity: a neurodevelopmental study. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[22]  I. Dziobek,et al.  Explaining altruistic sharing in the dictator game : The role of affective empathy, cognitive empathy, and justice sensitivity , 2013 .

[23]  M. Gollwitzer,et al.  The Justice Sensitivity Inventory: Factorial Validity, Location in the Personality Facet Space, Demographic Pattern, and Normative Data , 2010 .

[24]  M. Gollwitzer,et al.  Why and when justice sensitivity leads to pro- and antisocial behavior , 2009 .

[25]  G. Glover,et al.  Dissociable Intrinsic Connectivity Networks for Salience Processing and Executive Control , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[27]  J. Decety,et al.  The Role of the Right Temporoparietal Junction in Social Interaction: How Low-Level Computational Processes Contribute to Meta-Cognition , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[28]  Jan Gläscher,et al.  Visualization of Group Inference Data in Functional Neuroimaging , 2009, Neuroinformatics.

[29]  K. Pelphrey,et al.  Charting the typical and atypical development of the social brain , 2008, Development and Psychopathology.