Electroencephalography of response inhibition tasks: functional networks and cognitive contributions.
暂无分享,去创建一个
Christina F. Lavallee | René J. Huster | M. Falkenstein | C. Herrmann | R. Huster | S. Enriquez-Geppert | C. Lavallee
[1] M. Falkenstein,et al. Effects of task complexity on ERP components in Go/Nogo tasks. , 2013, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[2] René J. Huster,et al. Methods for Simultaneous EEG-fMRI: An Introductory Review , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[3] Marco Congedo,et al. Brain Oscillatory Activity during Spatial Navigation: Theta and Gamma Activity Link Medial Temporal and Parietal Regions , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[4] John J. B. Allen,et al. Theta lingua franca: a common mid-frontal substrate for action monitoring processes. , 2012, Psychophysiology.
[5] Nitin Tandon,et al. Roles for the pre-supplementary motor area and the right inferior frontal gyrus in stopping action: Electrophysiological responses and functional and structural connectivity , 2012, NeuroImage.
[6] G. Knyazev,et al. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews , 2012 .
[7] Janette L. Smith,et al. Conflict and inhibition in the cued-Go/NoGo task , 2011, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[8] R. Nigbur,et al. Theta power as a marker for cognitive interference , 2011, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[9] Markus Ullsperger,et al. Event-Related Potential Correlates of Performance-Monitoring in a Lateralized Time-Estimation Task , 2011, PloS one.
[10] Janette L. Smith,et al. On the use of event-related potentials to auditory stimuli in the Go/NoGo task , 2011, Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging.
[11] Robert T. Knight,et al. Electrophysiological Evidence for Different Inhibitory Mechanisms When Stopping or Changing a Planned Response , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[12] Andreas Mueller,et al. Dissociating action inhibition, conflict monitoring and sensory mismatch into independent components of event related potentials in GO/NOGO task , 2011, NeuroImage.
[13] A. Aron. From Reactive to Proactive and Selective Control: Developing a Richer Model for Stopping Inappropriate Responses , 2011, Biological Psychiatry.
[14] Christo Pantev,et al. Multimodal imaging of functional networks and event-related potentials in performance monitoring , 2011, NeuroImage.
[15] C. Basar-Eroglu,et al. Event-related delta and theta brain oscillations reflect age-related changes in both a general and a specific neuronal inhibitory mechanism , 2011, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[16] Diane Swick,et al. Are the neural correlates of stopping and not going identical? Quantitative meta-analysis of two response inhibition tasks , 2011, NeuroImage.
[17] Canan Basar-Eroglu,et al. Age-related increases in within-person variability: Delta and theta oscillations indicate that the elderly are not always old , 2011, Neuroscience Letters.
[18] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. Behavioral/systems/cognitive Effective Connectivity Reveals Important Roles for Both the Hyperdirect (fronto-subthalamic) and the Indirect (fronto-striatal-pallidal) Fronto-basal Ganglia Pathways during Response Inhibition , 2022 .
[19] J. Monterosso,et al. Different Forms of Self-Control Share a Neurocognitive Substrate , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[20] Aneta Brzezicka,et al. Short-term memory capacity (7±2) predicted by theta to gamma cycle length ratio , 2011, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.
[21] Katharine N. Thakkar,et al. Response Inhibition and Response Monitoring in a Saccadic Countermanding Task in Schizophrenia , 2011, Biological Psychiatry.
[22] Daniel H. Mathalon,et al. Frontally mediated inhibitory processing and white matter microstructure: age and alcoholism effects , 2010, Psychopharmacology.
[23] René J. Huster,et al. The role of the cingulate cortex as neural generator of the N200 and P300 in a tactile response inhibition task , 2010, Human brain mapping.
[24] W. Klimesch,et al. Control mechanisms in working memory: A possible function of EEG theta oscillations , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[25] Christo Pantev,et al. Conflict and inhibition differentially affect the N200/P300 complex in a combined go/nogo and stop-signal task , 2010, NeuroImage.
[26] Valerie Kirsch,et al. Separating distinct aspects of the voluntary selection between response alternatives: N2- and P3-related BOLD responses , 2010, NeuroImage.
[27] John Duncan,et al. The role of the right inferior frontal gyrus: inhibition and attentional control , 2010, NeuroImage.
[28] Yoshiharu Yamamoto,et al. Single-trial EEG Power and Phase Dynamics Associated with Voluntary Response Inhibition , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[29] A. Engel,et al. Beta-band oscillations—signalling the status quo? , 2010, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[30] Maneesh C. Patel,et al. Distinct frontal systems for response inhibition, attentional capture, and error processing , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[31] Andrew Heathcote,et al. Sequence effects support the conflict theory of N2 and P3 in the Go/NoGo task. , 2010, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[32] Borís Burle,et al. Mechanisms and Dynamics of Cortical Motor Inhibition in the Stop-signal Paradigm: A TMS Study , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[33] Frederick Verbruggen,et al. Having a goal to stop action is associated with advance control of specific motor representations , 2010, Neuropsychologia.
[34] Michael Falkenstein,et al. Response inhibition subprocesses and dopaminergic pathways: Basal ganglia disease effects , 2010, Neuropsychologia.
[35] Christo Pantev,et al. Individualized EEG source reconstruction of Stroop interference with masked color words , 2010, NeuroImage.
[36] René J. Huster,et al. Sex differences in cognitive control are associated with midcingulate and callosal morphology , 2010, Brain Structure and Function.
[37] C. Mulert,et al. Neural correlates (ERP/fMRI) of voluntary selection in adult ADHD patients , 2010, European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience.
[38] G. Buzsáki,et al. Theta Oscillations Provide Temporal Windows for Local Circuit Computation in the Entorhinal-Hippocampal Loop , 2009, Neuron.
[39] Peter Brown,et al. Boosting Cortical Activity at Beta-Band Frequencies Slows Movement in Humans , 2009, Current Biology.
[40] Michael A. DiSano,et al. Intracranial EEG Reveals a Time- and Frequency-Specific Role for the Right Inferior Frontal Gyrus and Primary Motor Cortex in Stopping Initiated Responses , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[41] Adam R. Aron. Introducing a special issue on stopping action and cognition , 2009, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[42] Derk-Jan Dijk,et al. Sleep function: current questions and new approaches , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.
[43] G. Tononi,et al. Source modeling sleep slow waves , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[44] Hiroki M. Morimoto,et al. Functional dissociation in right inferior frontal cortex during performance of go/no-go task. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.
[45] N. McNaughton,et al. Frontal-midline theta from the perspective of hippocampal “theta” , 2008, Progress in Neurobiology.
[46] G. Logan,et al. Response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[47] Darryl W. Schneider,et al. Automatic and Controlled Response Inhibition: Associative Learning in the Go/no-go and Stop-signal Paradigms the Go/no-go Paradigm and the Stop-signal Paradigm , 2022 .
[48] T. Klingberg,et al. Common and unique components of inhibition and working memory: An fMRI, within-subjects investigation , 2008, Neuropsychologia.
[49] Shuhei Yamaguchi,et al. The Key Locus of Common Response Inhibition Network for No-go and Stop Signals , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[50] Maximilian Reiser,et al. Influence of trait anxiety on inhibitory control in alcohol-dependent patients: simultaneous acquisition of ERPs and BOLD responses. , 2008, Journal of psychiatric research.
[51] Jonathan R. Folstein,et al. Novelty and conflict in the categorization of complex stimuli. , 2008, Psychophysiology.
[52] Wouter De Baene,et al. Stimulus ambiguity elicits response conflict , 2008, Neuroscience Letters.
[53] R. Barry,et al. Movement-related potentials in the Go/NoGo task: The P3 reflects both cognitive and motor inhibition , 2008, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[54] Stuart J. Johnstone,et al. Effects of varying stop-signal probability on ERPs in the stop-signal task: Do they reflect variations in inhibitory processing or simply novelty effects? , 2008, Biological Psychology.
[55] Philippe Boulinguez,et al. Warning signals induce automatic EMG activations and proactive volitional inhibition: evidence from analysis of error distribution in simple RT. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.
[56] J. Artieda,et al. Oscillatory changes related to the forced termination of a movement , 2008, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[57] S. Sünram-Lea,et al. P3b versus P3a: an event-related potential investigation of the glucose facilitation effect , 2008, Journal of psychopharmacology.
[58] G. Knyazev. Motivation, emotion, and their inhibitory control mirrored in brain oscillations , 2007, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[59] Gary Glover,et al. Frontostriatal connectivity and its role in cognitive control in parent-child dyads with ADHD. , 2007, The American journal of psychiatry.
[60] J. Polich. Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[61] O. Jensen,et al. Cross-frequency coupling between neuronal oscillations , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[62] Leanne Boucher,et al. Stopping eye and hand movements: Are the processes independent? , 2007, Perception & psychophysics.
[63] A. Aron. The Neural Basis of Inhibition in Cognitive Control , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.
[64] B. Sahakian,et al. The neuropsychiatry of impulsivity , 2007, Current opinion in psychiatry.
[65] N. Busch,et al. Gamma amplitudes are coupled to theta phase in human EEG during visual perception. , 2007, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[66] Robert J. Barry,et al. Response priming in the Go/NoGo task: The N2 reflects neither inhibition nor conflict , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[67] R. Barry,et al. The development of stop-signal and Go/Nogo response inhibition in children aged 7-12 years: performance and event-related potential indices. , 2007, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[68] Panteleimon Giannakopoulos,et al. Distinction between Perceptual and Attentional Processing in Working Memory Tasks: A Study of Phase-locked and Induced Oscillatory Brain Dynamics , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[69] Laura Busse,et al. Electrophysiological activity underlying inhibitory control processes in normal adults , 2006, Neuropsychologia.
[70] Robert J. Barry,et al. The auditory-evoked N2 and P3 components in the stop-signal task: Indices of inhibition, response-conflict or error-detection? , 2006, Brain and Cognition.
[71] R. Barry,et al. Effects of pre-stimulus processing on subsequent events in a warned Go/NoGo paradigm: response preparation, execution and inhibition. , 2006, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[72] W. Byblow,et al. Intracortical inhibition during volitional inhibition of prepared action. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.
[73] Allen Azizian,et al. Beware misleading cues: perceptual similarity modulates the N2/P3 complex. , 2006, Psychophysiology.
[74] Leslie S. Prichep,et al. Correlation of PET and qEEG in normal subjects , 2006, Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging.
[75] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. Effects of stop-signal modality on the N2/P3 complex elicited in the stop-signal paradigm , 2006, Biological Psychology.
[76] Craig J. Brozinsky,et al. Oscillatory EEG correlates of episodic trace decay. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.
[77] Jason B. Mattingley,et al. Executive “Brake Failure” following Deactivation of Human Frontal Lobe , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[78] T. Egner,et al. Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[79] C. C. Duncan,et al. Closed head injury-related information processing deficits: an event-related potential analysis. , 2005, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[80] Peter Brown,et al. Existing Motor State Is Favored at the Expense of New Movement during 13-35 Hz Oscillatory Synchrony in the Human Corticospinal System , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[81] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance. , 2005, Annual review of neuroscience.
[82] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Decision making, the P3, and the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.
[83] Kathryn M. McMillan,et al. A comparison of label‐based review and ALE meta‐analysis in the Stroop task , 2005, Human brain mapping.
[84] J Leon Kenemans,et al. Source analysis of the N2 in a cued Go/NoGo task. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[85] Maren Grigutsch,et al. EEG oscillations and wavelet analysis , 2005 .
[86] Koji Inui,et al. Effects of ISI and stimulus probability on event-related go/nogo potentials after somatosensory stimulation , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.
[87] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[88] D. Pizzagalli,et al. When ‘go’ and ‘nogo’ are equally frequent: ERP components and cortical tomography , 2004, The European journal of neuroscience.
[89] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. Effects of stop-signal probability in the stop-signal paradigm: The N2/P3 complex further validated , 2004, Brain and Cognition.
[90] Geert J. M. van Boxtel,et al. The N2 in go/no-go tasks reflects conflict monitoring not response inhibition , 2004, Brain and Cognition.
[91] Franck Vidal,et al. Physiological evidence for response inhibition in choice reaction time tasks , 2004, Brain and Cognition.
[92] R. McCarley,et al. The NoGo P300 ‘anteriorization’ effect and response inhibition , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[93] D. Munoz,et al. Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[94] Koji Inui,et al. Effects of a go/nogo task on event-related potentials following somatosensory stimulation , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[95] K Richard Ridderinkhof,et al. ERP components associated with successful and unsuccessful stopping in a stop-signal task. , 2004, Psychophysiology.
[96] Ahmet Ademoglu,et al. Wavelet Analysis of P3a and P3b , 2004, Brain Topography.
[97] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Stimulus modality, perceptual overlap, and the go/no-go N2. , 2004, Psychophysiology.
[98] G. Band,et al. Speed-accuracy modulation in case of conflict: the roles of activation and inhibition , 2003, Psychological research.
[99] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. Electrophysiological correlates of anterior cingulate function in a go/no-go task: Effects of response conflict and trial type frequency , 2003, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[100] Gordon D Logan,et al. Horse-race model simulations of the stop-signal procedure. , 2003, Acta psychologica.
[101] J. Polich. Theoretical Overview of P3a and P3b , 2003 .
[102] Michael D. Dodd,et al. In Opposition to Inhibition , 2003 .
[103] C. Carter,et al. The anterior cingulate as a conflict monitor: fMRI and ERP studies , 2002, Physiology & Behavior.
[104] A. A. Wijers,et al. Inhibition, response mode, and stimulus probability: a comparative event-related potential study , 2002, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[105] B. J. Casey,et al. The Effect of Preceding Context on Inhibition: An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2002, NeuroImage.
[106] P. Anderer,et al. Non-invasive electrophysiological neuroimaging of sleep , 2002 .
[107] Michael Falkenstein,et al. Inhibition-Related ERP Components: Variation with Modality, Age, and Time-on-Task , 2002 .
[108] H. Bokura,et al. Electrophysiological correlates for response inhibition in a Go/NoGo task , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[109] M. W. Molen,et al. A psychophysiological analysis of inhibitory motor control in the stop-signal paradigm , 2001, Biological Psychology.
[110] K. J. Bruin,et al. Response priming in a go/nogo task: do we have to explain the go/nogo N2 effect in terms of response activation instead of inhibition? , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[111] T. Braver,et al. Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of frequency, inhibition and errors. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.
[112] R. Knight,et al. Mechanisms of human attention: event-related potentials and oscillations , 2001, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[113] J. Shucard,et al. Stimulus modality and Go/NoGo effects on P3 during parallel visual and auditory continuous performance tasks. , 2001, Psychophysiology.
[114] E. Bullmore,et al. Mapping Motor Inhibition: Conjunctive Brain Activations across Different Versions of Go/No-Go and Stop Tasks , 2001, NeuroImage.
[115] M M Mesulam,et al. An electrophysiological index of stimulus unfamiliarity. , 2000, Psychophysiology.
[116] M. Hallett,et al. The relative metabolic demand of inhibition and excitation , 2000, Nature.
[117] H. Mayberg,et al. An ERP study of the temporal course of the Stroop color-word interference effect , 2000, Neuropsychologia.
[118] P. Apkarian,et al. Motoric response inhibition in finger movement and saccadic eye movement: a comparative study , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[119] J. Hohnsbein,et al. ERP components in Go/Nogo tasks and their relation to inhibition. , 1999, Acta psychologica.
[120] E. John,et al. Correlation of qEEG with PET in Schizophrenia , 1998, Neuropsychobiology.
[121] M Spitzer,et al. The time course of brain activations during response inhibition: evidence from event‐related potentials in a go/no go task , 1998, Neuroreport.
[122] J. Polich,et al. Stimulus context determines P3a and P3b. , 1998, Psychophysiology.
[123] T. Nakada,et al. A Review of EEG and Blood Flow Data , 1998, Reviews in the neurosciences.
[124] N. Takahashi,et al. Frontal Midline Theta Rhythm in Young Healthy Adults , 1997, Clinical EEG.
[125] B. Kopp,et al. N2, P3 and the lateralized readiness potential in a nogo task involving selective response priming. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[126] B. Kopp,et al. N200 in the flanker task as a neurobehavioral tool for investigating executive control. , 1996, Psychophysiology.
[127] G. Pfurtscheller,et al. Post-movement beta synchronization. A correlate of an idling motor area? , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[128] B. Vogt,et al. Human cingulate cortex: Surface features, flat maps, and cytoarchitecture , 1995, The Journal of comparative neurology.
[129] G D Logan,et al. Strategies and mechanisms in nonselective and selective inhibitory motor control. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[130] M G Coles,et al. A brain potential manifestation of error-related processing. , 1995, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.
[131] R. Knight,et al. Role of human prefrontal cortex in attention control. , 1995, Advances in neurology.
[132] J. Hohnsbein,et al. Effects of choice complexity on different subcomponents of the late positive complex of the event-related potential. , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[133] Frontal Midline Theta Rhythm: Differentiating the Physiological Theta Rhythm from the Abnormal Discharge , 1994, Clinical EEG.
[134] E. Schröger. Event-related potentials to auditory stimuli following transient shifts of spatial attention in a Go/Nogo task , 1993, Biological Psychology.
[135] Martin Eimer,et al. Effects of attention and stimulus probability on ERPs in a Go/Nogo task , 1993, Biological Psychology.
[136] E. Basar,et al. P300-response: possible psychophysiological correlates in delta and theta frequency channels. A review. , 1992, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[137] E. Jodo,et al. Relation of a negative ERP component to response inhibition in a Go/No-go task. , 1992, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[138] C M Michel,et al. Localization of the sources of EEG delta, theta, alpha and beta frequency bands using the FFT dipole approximation. , 1992, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[139] J. Hohnsbein,et al. Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[140] G. Logan,et al. In search of the point of no return: the control of response processes. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[141] G. Logan,et al. On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[142] R. Knight. Decreased response to novel stimuli after prefrontal lesions in man. , 1984, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[143] J. Gray,et al. Précis of The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system , 1982, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[144] H G Vaughan,et al. The scalp topography of potentials in auditory and visual Go/NoGo tasks. , 1977, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[145] G. McCarthy,et al. On the influence of task relevance and stimulus probability on event-related-potential components. , 1977, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[146] E. Courchesne,et al. Stimulus novelty, task relevance and the visual evoked potential in man. , 1975, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.