Electroencephalography of response inhibition tasks: functional networks and cognitive contributions.

[1]  M. Falkenstein,et al.  Effects of task complexity on ERP components in Go/Nogo tasks. , 2013, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[2]  René J. Huster,et al.  Methods for Simultaneous EEG-fMRI: An Introductory Review , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  Marco Congedo,et al.  Brain Oscillatory Activity during Spatial Navigation: Theta and Gamma Activity Link Medial Temporal and Parietal Regions , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[4]  John J. B. Allen,et al.  Theta lingua franca: a common mid-frontal substrate for action monitoring processes. , 2012, Psychophysiology.

[5]  Nitin Tandon,et al.  Roles for the pre-supplementary motor area and the right inferior frontal gyrus in stopping action: Electrophysiological responses and functional and structural connectivity , 2012, NeuroImage.

[6]  G. Knyazev,et al.  Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews , 2012 .

[7]  Janette L. Smith,et al.  Conflict and inhibition in the cued-Go/NoGo task , 2011, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[8]  R. Nigbur,et al.  Theta power as a marker for cognitive interference , 2011, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[9]  Markus Ullsperger,et al.  Event-Related Potential Correlates of Performance-Monitoring in a Lateralized Time-Estimation Task , 2011, PloS one.

[10]  Janette L. Smith,et al.  On the use of event-related potentials to auditory stimuli in the Go/NoGo task , 2011, Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging.

[11]  Robert T. Knight,et al.  Electrophysiological Evidence for Different Inhibitory Mechanisms When Stopping or Changing a Planned Response , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[12]  Andreas Mueller,et al.  Dissociating action inhibition, conflict monitoring and sensory mismatch into independent components of event related potentials in GO/NOGO task , 2011, NeuroImage.

[13]  A. Aron From Reactive to Proactive and Selective Control: Developing a Richer Model for Stopping Inappropriate Responses , 2011, Biological Psychiatry.

[14]  Christo Pantev,et al.  Multimodal imaging of functional networks and event-related potentials in performance monitoring , 2011, NeuroImage.

[15]  C. Basar-Eroglu,et al.  Event-related delta and theta brain oscillations reflect age-related changes in both a general and a specific neuronal inhibitory mechanism , 2011, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[16]  Diane Swick,et al.  Are the neural correlates of stopping and not going identical? Quantitative meta-analysis of two response inhibition tasks , 2011, NeuroImage.

[17]  Canan Basar-Eroglu,et al.  Age-related increases in within-person variability: Delta and theta oscillations indicate that the elderly are not always old , 2011, Neuroscience Letters.

[18]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Behavioral/systems/cognitive Effective Connectivity Reveals Important Roles for Both the Hyperdirect (fronto-subthalamic) and the Indirect (fronto-striatal-pallidal) Fronto-basal Ganglia Pathways during Response Inhibition , 2022 .

[19]  J. Monterosso,et al.  Different Forms of Self-Control Share a Neurocognitive Substrate , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  Aneta Brzezicka,et al.  Short-term memory capacity (7±2) predicted by theta to gamma cycle length ratio , 2011, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[21]  Katharine N. Thakkar,et al.  Response Inhibition and Response Monitoring in a Saccadic Countermanding Task in Schizophrenia , 2011, Biological Psychiatry.

[22]  Daniel H. Mathalon,et al.  Frontally mediated inhibitory processing and white matter microstructure: age and alcoholism effects , 2010, Psychopharmacology.

[23]  René J. Huster,et al.  The role of the cingulate cortex as neural generator of the N200 and P300 in a tactile response inhibition task , 2010, Human brain mapping.

[24]  W. Klimesch,et al.  Control mechanisms in working memory: A possible function of EEG theta oscillations , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[25]  Christo Pantev,et al.  Conflict and inhibition differentially affect the N200/P300 complex in a combined go/nogo and stop-signal task , 2010, NeuroImage.

[26]  Valerie Kirsch,et al.  Separating distinct aspects of the voluntary selection between response alternatives: N2- and P3-related BOLD responses , 2010, NeuroImage.

[27]  John Duncan,et al.  The role of the right inferior frontal gyrus: inhibition and attentional control , 2010, NeuroImage.

[28]  Yoshiharu Yamamoto,et al.  Single-trial EEG Power and Phase Dynamics Associated with Voluntary Response Inhibition , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[29]  A. Engel,et al.  Beta-band oscillations—signalling the status quo? , 2010, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[30]  Maneesh C. Patel,et al.  Distinct frontal systems for response inhibition, attentional capture, and error processing , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[31]  Andrew Heathcote,et al.  Sequence effects support the conflict theory of N2 and P3 in the Go/NoGo task. , 2010, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[32]  Borís Burle,et al.  Mechanisms and Dynamics of Cortical Motor Inhibition in the Stop-signal Paradigm: A TMS Study , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[33]  Frederick Verbruggen,et al.  Having a goal to stop action is associated with advance control of specific motor representations , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  Michael Falkenstein,et al.  Response inhibition subprocesses and dopaminergic pathways: Basal ganglia disease effects , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[35]  Christo Pantev,et al.  Individualized EEG source reconstruction of Stroop interference with masked color words , 2010, NeuroImage.

[36]  René J. Huster,et al.  Sex differences in cognitive control are associated with midcingulate and callosal morphology , 2010, Brain Structure and Function.

[37]  C. Mulert,et al.  Neural correlates (ERP/fMRI) of voluntary selection in adult ADHD patients , 2010, European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience.

[38]  G. Buzsáki,et al.  Theta Oscillations Provide Temporal Windows for Local Circuit Computation in the Entorhinal-Hippocampal Loop , 2009, Neuron.

[39]  Peter Brown,et al.  Boosting Cortical Activity at Beta-Band Frequencies Slows Movement in Humans , 2009, Current Biology.

[40]  Michael A. DiSano,et al.  Intracranial EEG Reveals a Time- and Frequency-Specific Role for the Right Inferior Frontal Gyrus and Primary Motor Cortex in Stopping Initiated Responses , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  Adam R. Aron Introducing a special issue on stopping action and cognition , 2009, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[42]  Derk-Jan Dijk,et al.  Sleep function: current questions and new approaches , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.

[43]  G. Tononi,et al.  Source modeling sleep slow waves , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[44]  Hiroki M. Morimoto,et al.  Functional dissociation in right inferior frontal cortex during performance of go/no-go task. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[45]  N. McNaughton,et al.  Frontal-midline theta from the perspective of hippocampal “theta” , 2008, Progress in Neurobiology.

[46]  G. Logan,et al.  Response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[47]  Darryl W. Schneider,et al.  Automatic and Controlled Response Inhibition: Associative Learning in the Go/no-go and Stop-signal Paradigms the Go/no-go Paradigm and the Stop-signal Paradigm , 2022 .

[48]  T. Klingberg,et al.  Common and unique components of inhibition and working memory: An fMRI, within-subjects investigation , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[49]  Shuhei Yamaguchi,et al.  The Key Locus of Common Response Inhibition Network for No-go and Stop Signals , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[50]  Maximilian Reiser,et al.  Influence of trait anxiety on inhibitory control in alcohol-dependent patients: simultaneous acquisition of ERPs and BOLD responses. , 2008, Journal of psychiatric research.

[51]  Jonathan R. Folstein,et al.  Novelty and conflict in the categorization of complex stimuli. , 2008, Psychophysiology.

[52]  Wouter De Baene,et al.  Stimulus ambiguity elicits response conflict , 2008, Neuroscience Letters.

[53]  R. Barry,et al.  Movement-related potentials in the Go/NoGo task: The P3 reflects both cognitive and motor inhibition , 2008, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[54]  Stuart J. Johnstone,et al.  Effects of varying stop-signal probability on ERPs in the stop-signal task: Do they reflect variations in inhibitory processing or simply novelty effects? , 2008, Biological Psychology.

[55]  Philippe Boulinguez,et al.  Warning signals induce automatic EMG activations and proactive volitional inhibition: evidence from analysis of error distribution in simple RT. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[56]  J. Artieda,et al.  Oscillatory changes related to the forced termination of a movement , 2008, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[57]  S. Sünram-Lea,et al.  P3b versus P3a: an event-related potential investigation of the glucose facilitation effect , 2008, Journal of psychopharmacology.

[58]  G. Knyazev Motivation, emotion, and their inhibitory control mirrored in brain oscillations , 2007, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[59]  Gary Glover,et al.  Frontostriatal connectivity and its role in cognitive control in parent-child dyads with ADHD. , 2007, The American journal of psychiatry.

[60]  J. Polich Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[61]  O. Jensen,et al.  Cross-frequency coupling between neuronal oscillations , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[62]  Leanne Boucher,et al.  Stopping eye and hand movements: Are the processes independent? , 2007, Perception & psychophysics.

[63]  A. Aron The Neural Basis of Inhibition in Cognitive Control , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[64]  B. Sahakian,et al.  The neuropsychiatry of impulsivity , 2007, Current opinion in psychiatry.

[65]  N. Busch,et al.  Gamma amplitudes are coupled to theta phase in human EEG during visual perception. , 2007, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[66]  Robert J. Barry,et al.  Response priming in the Go/NoGo task: The N2 reflects neither inhibition nor conflict , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[67]  R. Barry,et al.  The development of stop-signal and Go/Nogo response inhibition in children aged 7-12 years: performance and event-related potential indices. , 2007, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[68]  Panteleimon Giannakopoulos,et al.  Distinction between Perceptual and Attentional Processing in Working Memory Tasks: A Study of Phase-locked and Induced Oscillatory Brain Dynamics , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[69]  Laura Busse,et al.  Electrophysiological activity underlying inhibitory control processes in normal adults , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[70]  Robert J. Barry,et al.  The auditory-evoked N2 and P3 components in the stop-signal task: Indices of inhibition, response-conflict or error-detection? , 2006, Brain and Cognition.

[71]  R. Barry,et al.  Effects of pre-stimulus processing on subsequent events in a warned Go/NoGo paradigm: response preparation, execution and inhibition. , 2006, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[72]  W. Byblow,et al.  Intracortical inhibition during volitional inhibition of prepared action. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[73]  Allen Azizian,et al.  Beware misleading cues: perceptual similarity modulates the N2/P3 complex. , 2006, Psychophysiology.

[74]  Leslie S. Prichep,et al.  Correlation of PET and qEEG in normal subjects , 2006, Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging.

[75]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Effects of stop-signal modality on the N2/P3 complex elicited in the stop-signal paradigm , 2006, Biological Psychology.

[76]  Craig J. Brozinsky,et al.  Oscillatory EEG correlates of episodic trace decay. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[77]  Jason B. Mattingley,et al.  Executive “Brake Failure” following Deactivation of Human Frontal Lobe , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[78]  T. Egner,et al.  Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[79]  C. C. Duncan,et al.  Closed head injury-related information processing deficits: an event-related potential analysis. , 2005, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[80]  Peter Brown,et al.  Existing Motor State Is Favored at the Expense of New Movement during 13-35 Hz Oscillatory Synchrony in the Human Corticospinal System , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[81]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance. , 2005, Annual review of neuroscience.

[82]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Decision making, the P3, and the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.

[83]  Kathryn M. McMillan,et al.  A comparison of label‐based review and ALE meta‐analysis in the Stroop task , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[84]  J Leon Kenemans,et al.  Source analysis of the N2 in a cued Go/NoGo task. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[85]  Maren Grigutsch,et al.  EEG oscillations and wavelet analysis , 2005 .

[86]  Koji Inui,et al.  Effects of ISI and stimulus probability on event-related go/nogo potentials after somatosensory stimulation , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[87]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[88]  D. Pizzagalli,et al.  When ‘go’ and ‘nogo’ are equally frequent: ERP components and cortical tomography , 2004, The European journal of neuroscience.

[89]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Effects of stop-signal probability in the stop-signal paradigm: The N2/P3 complex further validated , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[90]  Geert J. M. van Boxtel,et al.  The N2 in go/no-go tasks reflects conflict monitoring not response inhibition , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[91]  Franck Vidal,et al.  Physiological evidence for response inhibition in choice reaction time tasks , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[92]  R. McCarley,et al.  The NoGo P300 ‘anteriorization’ effect and response inhibition , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[93]  D. Munoz,et al.  Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[94]  Koji Inui,et al.  Effects of a go/nogo task on event-related potentials following somatosensory stimulation , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[95]  K Richard Ridderinkhof,et al.  ERP components associated with successful and unsuccessful stopping in a stop-signal task. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[96]  Ahmet Ademoglu,et al.  Wavelet Analysis of P3a and P3b , 2004, Brain Topography.

[97]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Stimulus modality, perceptual overlap, and the go/no-go N2. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[98]  G. Band,et al.  Speed-accuracy modulation in case of conflict: the roles of activation and inhibition , 2003, Psychological research.

[99]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of anterior cingulate function in a go/no-go task: Effects of response conflict and trial type frequency , 2003, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[100]  Gordon D Logan,et al.  Horse-race model simulations of the stop-signal procedure. , 2003, Acta psychologica.

[101]  J. Polich Theoretical Overview of P3a and P3b , 2003 .

[102]  Michael D. Dodd,et al.  In Opposition to Inhibition , 2003 .

[103]  C. Carter,et al.  The anterior cingulate as a conflict monitor: fMRI and ERP studies , 2002, Physiology & Behavior.

[104]  A. A. Wijers,et al.  Inhibition, response mode, and stimulus probability: a comparative event-related potential study , 2002, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[105]  B. J. Casey,et al.  The Effect of Preceding Context on Inhibition: An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2002, NeuroImage.

[106]  P. Anderer,et al.  Non-invasive electrophysiological neuroimaging of sleep , 2002 .

[107]  Michael Falkenstein,et al.  Inhibition-Related ERP Components: Variation with Modality, Age, and Time-on-Task , 2002 .

[108]  H. Bokura,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates for response inhibition in a Go/NoGo task , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[109]  M. W. Molen,et al.  A psychophysiological analysis of inhibitory motor control in the stop-signal paradigm , 2001, Biological Psychology.

[110]  K. J. Bruin,et al.  Response priming in a go/nogo task: do we have to explain the go/nogo N2 effect in terms of response activation instead of inhibition? , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[111]  T. Braver,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of frequency, inhibition and errors. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[112]  R. Knight,et al.  Mechanisms of human attention: event-related potentials and oscillations , 2001, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[113]  J. Shucard,et al.  Stimulus modality and Go/NoGo effects on P3 during parallel visual and auditory continuous performance tasks. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[114]  E. Bullmore,et al.  Mapping Motor Inhibition: Conjunctive Brain Activations across Different Versions of Go/No-Go and Stop Tasks , 2001, NeuroImage.

[115]  M M Mesulam,et al.  An electrophysiological index of stimulus unfamiliarity. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[116]  M. Hallett,et al.  The relative metabolic demand of inhibition and excitation , 2000, Nature.

[117]  H. Mayberg,et al.  An ERP study of the temporal course of the Stroop color-word interference effect , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[118]  P. Apkarian,et al.  Motoric response inhibition in finger movement and saccadic eye movement: a comparative study , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[119]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  ERP components in Go/Nogo tasks and their relation to inhibition. , 1999, Acta psychologica.

[120]  E. John,et al.  Correlation of qEEG with PET in Schizophrenia , 1998, Neuropsychobiology.

[121]  M Spitzer,et al.  The time course of brain activations during response inhibition: evidence from event‐related potentials in a go/no go task , 1998, Neuroreport.

[122]  J. Polich,et al.  Stimulus context determines P3a and P3b. , 1998, Psychophysiology.

[123]  T. Nakada,et al.  A Review of EEG and Blood Flow Data , 1998, Reviews in the neurosciences.

[124]  N. Takahashi,et al.  Frontal Midline Theta Rhythm in Young Healthy Adults , 1997, Clinical EEG.

[125]  B. Kopp,et al.  N2, P3 and the lateralized readiness potential in a nogo task involving selective response priming. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[126]  B. Kopp,et al.  N200 in the flanker task as a neurobehavioral tool for investigating executive control. , 1996, Psychophysiology.

[127]  G. Pfurtscheller,et al.  Post-movement beta synchronization. A correlate of an idling motor area? , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[128]  B. Vogt,et al.  Human cingulate cortex: Surface features, flat maps, and cytoarchitecture , 1995, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[129]  G D Logan,et al.  Strategies and mechanisms in nonselective and selective inhibitory motor control. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[130]  M G Coles,et al.  A brain potential manifestation of error-related processing. , 1995, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.

[131]  R. Knight,et al.  Role of human prefrontal cortex in attention control. , 1995, Advances in neurology.

[132]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  Effects of choice complexity on different subcomponents of the late positive complex of the event-related potential. , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[133]  Frontal Midline Theta Rhythm: Differentiating the Physiological Theta Rhythm from the Abnormal Discharge , 1994, Clinical EEG.

[134]  E. Schröger Event-related potentials to auditory stimuli following transient shifts of spatial attention in a Go/Nogo task , 1993, Biological Psychology.

[135]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Effects of attention and stimulus probability on ERPs in a Go/Nogo task , 1993, Biological Psychology.

[136]  E. Basar,et al.  P300-response: possible psychophysiological correlates in delta and theta frequency channels. A review. , 1992, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[137]  E. Jodo,et al.  Relation of a negative ERP component to response inhibition in a Go/No-go task. , 1992, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[138]  C M Michel,et al.  Localization of the sources of EEG delta, theta, alpha and beta frequency bands using the FFT dipole approximation. , 1992, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[139]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components. II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[140]  G. Logan,et al.  In search of the point of no return: the control of response processes. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[141]  G. Logan,et al.  On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[142]  R. Knight Decreased response to novel stimuli after prefrontal lesions in man. , 1984, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[143]  J. Gray,et al.  Précis of The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system , 1982, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[144]  H G Vaughan,et al.  The scalp topography of potentials in auditory and visual Go/NoGo tasks. , 1977, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[145]  G. McCarthy,et al.  On the influence of task relevance and stimulus probability on event-related-potential components. , 1977, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[146]  E. Courchesne,et al.  Stimulus novelty, task relevance and the visual evoked potential in man. , 1975, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.