Role of form information in motion pooling and segmentation.

Traditional theories of visual perception have focused on either form or motion processing, implying a functional separation. However, increasing evidence indicates that these features interact at early stages of visual processing. The current study examined a well-known form-motion interaction, where a shape translates along a circular path behind opaque apertures, giving the impression of either independently translating lines (segmentation) or a globally coherent, translating shape. The purpose was to systemically examine how low-level motion information and form information interact to determine which percept is reported. To this end, we used a stimulus with boundaries comprising multiple, spatially-separated Gabor patches with three to eight sides. Results showed that shapes with four or fewer sides appeared to move in a segmented manner, whereas those with more sides were integrated as a solid shape. The separation between directions, rather than the total number of sides, causes this switch between integrated or segmented percepts. We conclude that the change between integration and segmentation depends on whether local motion directions can be independently resolved. We also reconcile previous results on the influence of shape closure on motion integration: Shapes that form open contours cause segmentation, but with no corresponding enhanced sensitivity for shapes forming closed contours. Overall, our results suggest that the resolution of the local motion signal determines whether motion segmentation or integration is perceived with only a small overall influence of form.

[1]  J Edwin Dickinson,et al.  Selective attention contributes to global processing in vision. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[2]  Mark Edwards,et al.  Motion streaks improve motion detection , 2007, Vision Research.

[3]  David R. Badcock,et al.  No interaction of first- and second-order signals in the extraction of global-motion and optic-flow , 2011, Vision Research.

[4]  E. Adelson,et al.  Phenomenal coherence of moving visual patterns , 1982, Nature.

[5]  R. Turner,et al.  Form and motion coherence activate independent, but not dorsal/ventral segregated, networks in the human brain , 2000, Current Biology.

[6]  Jean Lorenceau,et al.  Superposition catastrophe and form-motion binding. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[7]  D. Burr,et al.  Spatial and temporal selectivity of the human motion detection system , 1985, Vision Research.

[8]  M. Shiffrar,et al.  Increased Motion Linking Across Edges with Decreased Luminance Contrast, Edge Width and Duration , 1996, Vision Research.

[9]  G. Johansson Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .

[10]  E H Adelson,et al.  Beyond Junctions: Nonlocal form Constraints on Motion Interpretation , 2001, Perception.

[11]  Steven C Dakin,et al.  The aperture problem in contoured stimuli. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[12]  Edward H. Adelson,et al.  Motion Perception and Mid-Level Vision , 2003 .

[13]  S. Palmer,et al.  A century of Gestalt psychology in visual perception: I. Perceptual grouping and figure-ground organization. , 2012, Psychological bulletin.

[14]  K. D. De Valois,et al.  Vernier acuity with stationary moving Gabors. , 1991, Vision research.

[15]  Jeounghoon Kim,et al.  Dependence of plaid motion coherence on component grating directions , 1993, Vision Research.

[16]  David Alais,et al.  Local and global factors affecting the coherent motion of gratings presented in multiple apertures , 1998, Vision Research.

[17]  Maggie Shiffrar,et al.  The influence of terminators on motion integration across space , 1992, Vision Research.

[18]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  Computational models of cortical visual processing. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[19]  Peter W. McOwan,et al.  Motion transparency arises from perceptual grouping: evidence from luminance and contrast modulation motion displays , 1996, Current Biology.

[20]  Allison M McKendrick,et al.  Pattern cues disambiguate perceived direction in simple moving stimuli , 2003, Vision Research.

[21]  Peter U. Tse,et al.  Neural correlates of transformational apparent motion , 2006, NeuroImage.

[22]  Shin'ya Nishida,et al.  Advancement of motion psychophysics: review 2001-2010. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[23]  Alan L. F. Lee,et al.  A comparison of global motion perception using a multiple-aperture stimulus. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[24]  G Johansson,et al.  Spatio-temporal differentiation and integration in visual motion perception , 1976, Psychological research.

[25]  John Ross,et al.  Direct Evidence That “Speedlines” Influence Motion Mechanisms , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  John A. Greenwood,et al.  An extension of the transparent-motion detection limit using speed-tuned global-motion systems , 2006, Vision Research.

[27]  Tatsuto Takeuchi,et al.  PII: S0042-6989(98)00019-4 , 1998 .

[28]  E H Adelson,et al.  Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[29]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[30]  S. Nishida,et al.  Spatial-frequency tuning in the pooling of one- and two-dimensional motion signals , 2009, Vision Research.

[31]  R. Andersen,et al.  Transparent motion perception as detection of unbalanced motion signals. II. Physiology , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[32]  Mark Edwards,et al.  The detection of multiple global directions: capacity limits with spatially segregated and transparent-motion signals. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[33]  D. G. Albrecht,et al.  Spatial frequency selectivity of cells in macaque visual cortex , 1982, Vision Research.

[34]  William Curran,et al.  Directional performance in motion transparency , 2002, Vision Research.

[35]  David R. Badcock,et al.  Position encoding of the centres of global structure: Separate form and motion processes , 2009, Vision Research.

[36]  Kazushi Maruya,et al.  Spatial pooling of one-dimensional second-order motion signals. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[37]  Wilson S. Geisler,et al.  Motion streaks provide a spatial code for motion direction , 1999, Nature.

[38]  Edward H. Adelson,et al.  Motion illusions as optimal percepts , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[39]  S. Nishida,et al.  Effect of form cues on 1D and 2D motion pooling , 2013, Vision Research.

[40]  Jeounghoon Kim,et al.  Direction repulsion between components in motion transparency , 1996, Vision Research.

[41]  Jean Lorenceau,et al.  Form constraints in motion binding , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[42]  Matthew F. Tang,et al.  The broad orientation dependence of the motion streak aftereffect reveals interactions between form and motion neurons. , 2015, Journal of vision.

[43]  Bertrand Thirion,et al.  Perceptual alternations between unbound moving contours and bound shape motion engage a ventral/dorsal interplay. , 2012, Journal of vision.

[44]  D. Samuel Schwarzkopf,et al.  Direct evidence for encoding of motion streaks in human visual cortex , 2013, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[45]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Object vision and spatial vision: two cortical pathways , 1983, Trends in Neurosciences.

[46]  David R. Badcock,et al.  Second-order orientation cues to the axis of motion , 2009, Vision Research.

[47]  J Edwin Dickinson,et al.  Further evidence that local cues to shape in RF patterns are integrated globally. , 2012, Journal of vision.

[48]  A. Watson,et al.  Quest: A Bayesian adaptive psychometric method , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[49]  Matthew F. Tang,et al.  The shape of motion perception: global pooling of transformational apparent motion. , 2013, Journal of vision.

[50]  David Kane,et al.  Quantifying "the aperture problem" for judgments of motion direction in natural scenes. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[51]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[52]  Wilson S. Geisler,et al.  Contour grouping: closure effects are explained by good continuation and proximity , 2004, Vision Research.

[53]  S. Nishida,et al.  Adaptive pooling of visual motion signals by the human visual system revealed with a novel multi-element stimulus. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[54]  David R. Badcock,et al.  Coherent global motion in the absence of coherent velocity signals , 2000, Current Biology.

[55]  DH Hubel,et al.  Psychophysical evidence for separate channels for the perception of form, color, movement, and depth , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.