Discrete visual samples may control locomotor equilibrium and foot positioning in man.

The static or dynamic visual cues required for equilibrium as well as for foot guidance in visually guided locomotion in man were studied using a variety of locomotion supports and illumination and visual conditions. Stroboscopic illumination (brief flashes) and intermittent lighting (longer flashes) were used to control and to vary the visual sampling frequency of static (positional/orientational) visual cues. There were three main findings: First, visual control of foot positioning during locomotion over a narrow support depends mainly upon the availability of high frequency static visual cues (up to about 12 Hz); and third, static visual cues required for equilibrium control are extracted from both the peripheral and the central visual field. Assuming that discrete demands for feedback occur, a simple probabilistic model was proposed, according to which the mean time that elapses following presentation of static visual cues about positions or changes of position accounts for the differences in the difficulty of the various illumination conditions.

[1]  [Perception of the velocity of intermittent light]. , 1971, Canadian journal of psychology.

[2]  C. Tyler Temporal characteristics in apparent movement: omega movement vs. phi movement. , 1973, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[3]  A. R. Fregly Vestibular Ataxia and its Measurement in Man , 1975 .

[4]  G. Johansson Studies on Visual Perception of Locomotion , 1977, Perception.

[5]  David N. Lee,et al.  Visual control of locomotion. , 1977, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[6]  J. Dichgans,et al.  Visual-Vestibular Interaction: Effects on Self-Motion Perception and Postural Control , 1978 .

[7]  T Caelli,et al.  Frequency, Phase, and Colour Coding in Apparent Motion: 2 , 1979, Perception.

[8]  B. Amblard,et al.  Role of Foveal and Peripheral Visual Information in Maintenance of Postural Equilibrium in Man , 1980, Perceptual and motor skills.

[9]  J. Thomson How do we use visual information to control locomotion? , 1980, Trends in Neurosciences.

[10]  A Straube,et al.  Visual stabilization of posture. Physiological stimulus characteristics and clinical aspects. , 1984, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  C. M. Schor,et al.  Optokinetic and vection responses to apparent motion in man , 1984, Vision Research.

[12]  Ken Nakayama,et al.  Biological image motion processing: A review , 1985, Vision Research.

[13]  B. Amblard,et al.  Orientation versus motion visual cues to control sensorimotor skills in some acrobatic leaps , 1985 .

[14]  O J Braddick,et al.  Temporal Properties of the Short-Range Process in Apparent Motion , 1985, Perception.

[15]  T. Stoffregen Flow structure versus retinal location in the optical control of stance. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  J. Thomson Intermittent versus continuous visual control: a reply to Elliott. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  W. H. Warren,et al.  Visual control of step length during running over irregular terrain. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  A. Delorme,et al.  Roles of retinal periphery and depth periphery in linear vection and visual control of standing in humans. , 1986, Canadian journal of psychology.

[19]  D. Elliott,et al.  The Influence of Premovement Visual Information on Manual Aiming , 1987, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[20]  J. Crémieux,et al.  Visual and vestibular control of locomotion in early and late sensory-deprived cats. , 1988, Progress in brain research.