A review and critical analysis of how cognitive neuroscientific investigations using dance can contribute to sport psychology

Whether watching Michael Jackson moonwalk or Savion Glover tap dance, it is striking how skilfully some people can move their bodies. The emerging field of cognitive neuroscience has produced important advances in understanding the control and perception of complex action. Here we outline the merits and limitations of neuroscience methods for studying psychological states and how they might inform sport psychology research. To do so, we review studies that have used dance paradigms, as well as summarize a debate regarding the utility of brain-based measurements for studying human cognition. Our central argument is twofold. First, the origins of studying dance with cognitive neuroscientific methods do not stem from a desire to inform dancers or instructors how to influence performance. Rather, dance is a useful tool to investigate the neurocognitive mechanisms that mediate the perception of complex action and development of expertise. In other words, neuroscientists were initially interested in ways that dance could be used to study elementary links between action and perception. Second, biological-level descriptions should not hold a privileged status over any other measure of a psychological state, and we urge consideration of the limits of brain-based methods when using cognitive neuroscientific approaches to understand the psychology of sport.

[1]  Blake W. Johnson,et al.  Mu rhythm modulation during observation of an object-directed grasp. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[2]  Emily S. Cross,et al.  Robotic movement preferentially engages the action observation network , 2012, Human brain mapping.

[3]  Deena Skolnick Weisberg,et al.  The Seductive Allure of Neuroscience Explanations , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[4]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Topological inference for EEG and MEG , 2010, 1011.2901.

[5]  R. Passingham,et al.  Seeing or Doing? Influence of Visual and Motor Familiarity in Action Observation , 2006, Current Biology.

[6]  M. Page,et al.  What Can't Functional Neuroimaging Tell the Cognitive Psychologist? , 2006, Cortex.

[7]  Karl J. Friston Functional and Effective Connectivity: A Review , 2011, Brain Connect..

[8]  R. Goebel,et al.  The Dynamics of Interhemispheric Compensatory Processes in Mental Imagery , 2005, Science.

[9]  J. Decety,et al.  Neural mechanisms subserving the perception of human actions , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[10]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Inside the brain of an elite athlete: the neural processes that support high achievement in sports , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[11]  A. Williams,et al.  What is modelled during observational learning? , 2007, Journal of sports sciences.

[12]  Frank Pollick,et al.  Dance and emotion in posterior parietal cortex: A low-frequency rTMS study , 2012, Brain Stimulation.

[13]  D. Spalding The Principles of Psychology , 1873, Nature.

[14]  M. Coltheart What has Functional Neuroimaging told us about the Mind (so far)? (Position Paper Presented to the European Cognitive Neuropsychology Workshop, Bressanone, 2005) , 2006, Cortex.

[15]  Lutz Jäncke,et al.  Structural neuroplasticity in the sensorimotor network of professional female ballet dancers , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[16]  R. Mausfeld On Some Unwarranted Tacit Assumptions in Cognitive Neuroscience† , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[17]  M. Grosbras,et al.  In the here and now: Enhanced motor corticospinal excitability in novices when watching live compared to video recorded dance , 2013, Cognitive neuroscience.

[18]  Emily S. Cross,et al.  Dissociable substrates for body motion and physical experience in the human action observation network , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.

[19]  P. Jacob What Do Mirror Neurons Contribute to Human Social Cognition , 2008 .

[20]  A F Sanders,et al.  Some aspects of reaction processes. , 1967, Acta psychologica.

[21]  Arnaud Badets,et al.  Intention in motor learning through observation , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[22]  A. Goldman Mirroring, Simulating and Mindreading , 2009 .

[23]  D. Wade,et al.  The effects of mental practice in stroke rehabilitation: a systematic review. , 2006, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[24]  Richard Henson,et al.  What has (Neuro)Psychology told us About the Mind (so Far)? a Reply to Coltheart (2006) , 2006, Cortex.

[25]  Emily S. Cross,et al.  Action observation in the infant brain: The role of body form and motion , 2012, Social neuroscience.

[26]  A. T. Welford,et al.  The fundamentals of skill , 1968 .

[27]  Simone Rossi,et al.  TMS in cognitive plasticity and the potential for rehabilitation , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[28]  Trevor Thompson,et al.  EEG applications for sport and performance. , 2008, Methods.

[29]  J. Decety,et al.  The power of simulation: Imagining one's own and other's behavior , 2006, Brain Research.

[30]  A. Sanders Towards a model of stress and human performance. , 1983, Acta psychologica.

[31]  Sensorimotor Transformations in the Posterior Parietal Cortex , 2004 .

[32]  Yongmin Chang,et al.  Brain activation patterns of motor imagery reflect plastic changes associated with intensive shooting training , 2012, Behavioural Brain Research.

[33]  L. Craighero,et al.  Corticospinal excitability is specifically modulated by motor imagery: a magnetic stimulation study , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  Emily S. Cross,et al.  Contorted and ordinary body postures in the human brain , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[35]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation during Positron Emission Tomography: A New Method for Studying Connectivity of the Human Cerebral Cortex , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[37]  R. Henson What can Functional Neuroimaging Tell the Experimental Psychologist? , 2005, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[38]  Alan D. Castel,et al.  Seeing is believing: The effect of brain images on judgments of scientific reasoning , 2008, Cognition.

[39]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[40]  F. Pollick,et al.  Arousal decrease in ‘Sleeping Beauty’: audiences’ neurophysiological correlates to watching a narrative dance performance of 2.5 hrs. , 2011 .

[41]  W. James,et al.  The Principles of Psychology. , 1983 .

[42]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action recognition in the premotor cortex. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[43]  V B Mountcastle,et al.  The view from within: pathways to the study of perception. , 1975, The Johns Hopkins medical journal.

[44]  Y. Marignac,et al.  Note , 1951, Neurochemistry International.

[45]  Frank E. Pollick,et al.  Motor Simulation without Motor Expertise: Enhanced Corticospinal Excitability in Visually Experienced Dance Spectators , 2012, PloS one.

[46]  Scott H. Johnson-Frey Stimulation through simulation? Motor imagery and functional reorganization in hemiplegic stroke patients , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[47]  Peter Kirsch,et al.  Differential activation of pre-SMA and SMA proper during action observation: Effects of instructions , 2005, NeuroImage.

[48]  V. Mountcastle,et al.  Visual input to the visuomotor mechanisms of the monkey's parietal lobe. , 1977, Science.

[49]  E. McAuley Modeling and Self-Efficacy: A Test of Bandura's Model , 1985 .

[50]  D. Massaro,et al.  Models of integration given multiple sources of information. , 1990, Psychological review.

[51]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Parietal Lobe: From Action Organization to Intention Understanding , 2005, Science.

[52]  A. Moran Cognitive psychology in sport: Progress and prospects☆ , 2009 .

[53]  Marc Jeannerod,et al.  Putting oneself in the perspective of the other: A framework for self–other differentiation , 2008, Social neuroscience.

[54]  C. Heyes,et al.  Science Perspectives on Psychological Mirror Neuron Forum on Behalf Of: Association for Psychological Science , 2022 .

[55]  M. Jeannerod Neural Simulation of Action: A Unifying Mechanism for Motor Cognition , 2001, NeuroImage.

[56]  V. Mountcastle,et al.  Posterior parietal association cortex of the monkey: command functions for operations within extrapersonal space. , 1975, Journal of neurophysiology.

[57]  R. DeCharms Applications of real-time fMRI , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[58]  A Mark Williams,et al.  Current status of observational learning research and the role of demonstrations in sport , 2007, Journal of sports sciences.

[59]  C. Miniussi,et al.  Combining TMS and EEG Offers New Prospects in Cognitive Neuroscience , 2009, Brain Topography.

[60]  A. Guillot,et al.  Re-imagining motor imagery: building bridges between cognitive neuroscience and sport psychology. , 2012, British journal of psychology.

[61]  H. Karnath,et al.  Using human brain lesions to infer function: a relic from a past era in the fMRI age? , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[62]  A. Bandura Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory , 1985 .

[63]  Emily S. Cross,et al.  Building a motor simulation de novo: Observation of dance by dancers , 2006, NeuroImage.

[64]  Emily S. Cross,et al.  The Impact of Aesthetic Evaluation and Physical Ability on Dance Perception , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[65]  Aljoscha C. Neubauer,et al.  Brain correlates underlying creative thinking: EEG alpha activity in professional vs. novice dancers , 2009, NeuroImage.

[66]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: interpretations and misinterpretations , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[67]  D. Landers,et al.  Enhancing Self-efficacy in High-avoidance Motor Tasks: A Comparison of Modeling Techniques , 1979 .

[68]  R. Baumeister,et al.  Social exclusion impairs self-regulation. , 2005, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[69]  Done-Sik Yoo,et al.  Neural correlates related to action observation in expert archers , 2011, Behavioural Brain Research.

[70]  A. Guillot,et al.  The neurophysiological foundations of mental and motor imagery , 2010 .

[71]  Isbn The Principles of Psychology: Volume 1 , 2004 .

[72]  J. Morton Understanding Developmental Disorders: A Causal Modelling Approach , 2005 .

[73]  J. Kilner,et al.  Bias in a common EEG and MEG statistical analysis and how to avoid it , 2013, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[74]  F. Pollick,et al.  Uni- and multisensory brain areas are synchronised across spectators when watching unedited dance recordings , 2013, i-Perception.

[75]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[76]  D. E. Glaser,et al.  Towards a sensorimotor aesthetics of performing art , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.

[77]  S. Bennett,et al.  Theory and practice in sport psychology and motor behaviour needs to be constrained by integrative modelling of brain and behaviour , 2000, Journal of sports sciences.

[78]  Shigeru Sato,et al.  Effect of motion smoothness on brain activity while observing a dance: An fMRI study using a humanoid robot , 2010, Social neuroscience.

[79]  L. Barsalou Grounded cognition. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.

[80]  Emily S. Cross,et al.  Sensitivity of the action observation network to physical and observational learning. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[81]  C. Urgesi,et al.  Action anticipation and motor resonance in elite basketball players , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[82]  Michael J. Martinez,et al.  Cerebral Cortex doi:10.1093/cercor/bhj057 Cerebral Cortex Advance Access published October 12, 2005 The Neural Basis of Human Dance , 2022 .

[83]  Emily S. Cross,et al.  The influence of visual training on predicting complex action sequences , 2013, Human brain mapping.

[84]  J. Morton Understanding Developmental Disorders , 2004 .

[85]  F. Pollick,et al.  Arousal Decrease in Sleeping Beauty: Audiences' Neurophysiological Correlates to Watching a Narrative Dance Performance of two-and-a-half hours , 2011 .

[86]  W. Bechtel Decomposing the Mind-Brain: A Long-Term Pursuit , 2002 .

[87]  V. Gallese,et al.  What is so special about embodied simulation? , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[88]  A. Tachibana,et al.  Parietal and temporal activity during a multimodal dance video game: An fNIRS study , 2011, Neuroscience Letters.

[89]  Simon B Eickhoff,et al.  Brain regions involved in human movement perception: A quantitative voxel‐based meta‐analysis , 2012, Human brain mapping.

[90]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[91]  P. Haggard,et al.  Extrastriate body area underlies aesthetic evaluation of body stimuli , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[92]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Visual mental imagery induces retinotopically organized activation of early visual areas. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[93]  R. Passingham,et al.  Action observation and acquired motor skills: an FMRI study with expert dancers. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[94]  M. Heil,et al.  Expertise in dance modulates alpha/beta event‐related desynchronization during action observation , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.

[95]  Marina Schmid,et al.  An Introduction To The Event Related Potential Technique , 2016 .

[96]  E Macaluso,et al.  Action anticipation beyond the action observation network: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study in expert basketball players , 2012, The European journal of neuroscience.

[97]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[98]  R. Poldrack Inferring Mental States from Neuroimaging Data: From Reverse Inference to Large-Scale Decoding , 2011, Neuron.

[99]  Sean M. Polyn,et al.  Beyond mind-reading: multi-voxel pattern analysis of fMRI data , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[100]  A. Guillot,et al.  An emerging paradigm: a strength-based approach to exploring mental imagery , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[101]  J. Schoffelen,et al.  Nonparametric statistical testing of coherence differences , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[102]  Emily S. Cross,et al.  Neurocognitive control in dance perception and performance. , 2012, Acta psychologica.

[103]  David E. J. Linden,et al.  Combining transcranial magnetic stimulation and functional imaging in cognitive brain research: possibilities and limitations , 2003, Brain Research Reviews.

[104]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Localization of grasp representations in humans by positron emission tomography , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[105]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Brain regions with mirror properties: A meta-analysis of 125 human fMRI studies , 2012, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.