Cuttlefish adjust body pattern intensity with respect to substrate intensity to aid camouflage, but do not camouflage in extremely low light

Abstract Cuttlefish are able to camouflage to a wide variety of natural backgrounds that contain varying colors, intensities and patterns. Numerous studies have investigated the visual cues that influence cuttlefish body pattern expression, yet none have addressed experimentally how well overall intensity is matched between animal and substrate. Here, cuttlefish were tested on artificial and natural substrates that varied in intensity and were illuminated by different light levels; calibrated grayscale photographs were used to analyze the intensity of cuttlefish and their surrounding substrates. We found that cuttlefish scaled their body pattern intensity with respect to substrate intensity under bright and moderate lighting conditions, but not under low or extremely low lighting conditions. Surprisingly, in extremely low light (

[1]  Raymond C. Smith,et al.  Measurements of spectral irradiance underwater , 1970 .

[2]  Justine J Allen,et al.  The use of background matching vs. masquerade for camouflage in cuttlefish Sepia officinalis , 2011, Vision Research.

[3]  R. Hanlon,et al.  Malleable skin coloration in cephalopods: selective reflectance, transmission and absorbance of light by chromatophores and iridophores , 2007, Cell and Tissue Research.

[4]  S. Johnsen,et al.  Hidden in Plain Sight: The Ecology and Physiology of Organismal Transparency , 2001, The Biological Bulletin.

[5]  L. Dickel,et al.  Effects of rearing conditions on sand digging efficiency in juvenile cuttlefish , 2004, Behavioural Processes.

[6]  Roger T Hanlon,et al.  Hyperspectral imaging of cuttlefish camouflage indicates good color match in the eyes of fish predators , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[7]  Roger T Hanlon,et al.  Mechanisms and behavioural functions of structural coloration in cephalopods , 2009, Journal of The Royal Society Interface.

[8]  Erik Reinhard,et al.  Color imaging , 2009, SIGGRAPH '09.

[9]  S. Johnsen The Optics of Life: A Biologist's Guide to Light in Nature , 2011 .

[10]  Roger T. Hanlon,et al.  Color matching on natural substrates in cuttlefish, Sepia officinalis , 2008, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[11]  R. Williamson,et al.  Ontogenetic changes in the visual acuity of Sepia officinalis measured using the optomotor response , 2005 .

[12]  Roger T. Hanlon,et al.  Evidence for distributed light sensing in the skin of cuttlefish, Sepia officinalis , 2010, Biology Letters.

[13]  Justine J Allen,et al.  Night vision by cuttlefish enables changeable camouflage , 2010, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[14]  N. Marshall,et al.  To Be Seen or to Hide: Visual Characteristics of Body Patterns for Camouflage and Communication in the Australian Giant Cuttlefish Sepia apama , 2011, The American Naturalist.

[15]  J. Lythgoe The Ecology of vision , 1979 .

[16]  C. Chiao,et al.  Rapid Adaptive Camouflage in Cephalopods , 2011 .

[17]  T. Wardill,et al.  Neural control of tuneable skin iridescence in squid , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[18]  Roger T Hanlon,et al.  Use of commercial off-the-shelf digital cameras for scientific data acquisition and scene-specific color calibration. , 2014, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[19]  R. Hanlon,et al.  Adaptable Night Camouflage by Cuttlefish , 2007, The American Naturalist.

[20]  Derya Akkaynak,et al.  Quantification of cuttlefish (Sepiaofficinalis) camouflage: a study of color and luminance using in situ spectrometry , 2013, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[21]  J. Nicol,et al.  A survey of reflectivity in silvery teleosts , 1966, Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom.

[22]  Roger T. Hanlon,et al.  Color blindness and contrast perception in cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis) determined by a visual sensorimotor assay , 2006, Vision Research.

[23]  Justine J Allen,et al.  How does the blue-ringed octopus (Hapalochlaena lunulata) flash its blue rings? , 2012, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[24]  E. Florey,et al.  Electrical and mechanical responses of chromatophore muscle fibers of the squid, Loligo opalescens, to nerve stimulation and drugs , 1969, Zeitschrift für vergleichende Physiologie.

[25]  P. Jereb,et al.  Cephalopods of the world. An annotated and illustrated catalogue of cephalopod species known to date. Volume 1. Chambered nautiluses and sepioids (Nautilidae, Sepiidae, Sepiadariidae, Idiosepiidae and Spirulidae) , 2005 .

[26]  N. Justin Marshall,et al.  A fish-eye view of cuttlefish camouflage using in situ spectrometry , 2013 .

[27]  Roger Hanlon,et al.  Cephalopod dynamic camouflage , 2007, Current Biology.

[28]  Eric J Warrant,et al.  Vision in the deep sea , 2004, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.

[29]  C Chubb,et al.  Cephalopod dynamic camouflage: bridging the continuum between background matching and disruptive coloration , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[30]  H. B. Cott,et al.  Adaptive Coloration in Animals , 1940 .

[31]  Á. Guerra ECOLOGY OF SEPIA OFFICINALIS , 2006 .

[32]  T. Wardill,et al.  Chromatophore radial muscle fibers anchor in flexible squid skin , 2013 .

[33]  N. J. Marshall,et al.  Colour-blind camouflage , 1996, Nature.

[34]  R. Hanlon,et al.  Adaptive Coloration in Young Cuttlefish (Sepia Officinalis L.): The Morphology and Development of Body Patterns and Their Relation to Behaviour , 1988 .