Effect of language proficiency on proactive occulo-motor control among bilinguals

We examined the effect of language proficiency on the status and dynamics of proactive inhibitory control in an occulo-motor cued go-no-go task. The first experiment was designed to demonstrate the effect of second language proficiency on proactive inhibitory cost and adjustments in control by evaluating previous trial effects. This was achieved by introducing uncertainty about the upcoming event (go or no-go stimulus). High- and low- proficiency Hindi-English bilingual adults participated in the study. Saccadic latencies and errors were taken as the measures of performance. The results demonstrate a significantly lower proactive inhibitory cost and better up-regulation of proactive control under uncertainty among high- proficiency bilinguals. An analysis based on previous trial effects suggests that high- proficiency bilinguals were found to be better at releasing inhibition and adjustments in control, in an ongoing response activity in the case of uncertainty. To further understand the dynamics of proactive inhibitory control as a function of proficiency, the second experiment was designed to test the default versus temporary state hypothesis of proactive inhibitory control. Certain manipulations were introduced in the cued go-no-go task in order to make the upcoming go or no-go trial difficult to predict, which increased the demands on the implementation and maintenance of proactive control. High- proficiency bilinguals were found to rely on a default state of proactive inhibitory control whereas low- proficiency bilinguals were found to rely on temporary/transient proactive inhibition. Language proficiency, as one of the measures of bilingualism, was found to influence proactive inhibitory control and appears to modulate the dynamics of proactive inhibitory control.

[1]  Ellen Bialystok,et al.  Executive processes in appearance-reality tasks: the role of inhibition of attention and symbolic representation. , 2004, Child development.

[2]  Virginia Valian Bilingualism and cognition , 2014, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.

[3]  J. Altarriba,et al.  The effects of first- and second-language proficiency on conflict resolution and goal maintenance in bilinguals: Evidence from reaction time distributional analyses in a Stroop task* , 2012, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.

[4]  Matthew D. Hilchey,et al.  Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[5]  A. Vivas,et al.  Attentional processes in low-socioeconomic status bilingual children: are they modulated by the amount of bilingual experience? , 2015, Child development.

[6]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[7]  T. Bak,et al.  Bilingualism delays age at onset of dementia, independent of education and immigration status , 2013, Neurology.

[8]  John Ashburner,et al.  Structural plasticity in the bilingual brain: Proficiency in a second language and age at acquisition affect grey-matter density. , 2004 .

[9]  Philippe Boulinguez,et al.  Warning signals induce automatic EMG activations and proactive volitional inhibition: evidence from analysis of error distribution in simple RT. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  A. Murthy,et al.  Programming of double-step saccade sequences: Modulation by cognitive control , 2004, Vision Research.

[11]  R. Mishra,et al.  Does Language Proficiency Modulate Oculomotor Control? Evidence from Hindi-English Bilinguals. , 2012 .

[12]  A. Ardila,et al.  The effect of language proficiency on executive functions in balanced and unbalanced Spanish–English bilinguals * , 2016 .

[13]  Albert Costa,et al.  Lexical Access in Bilingual Speech Production: Evidence from Language Switching in Highly Proficient Bilinguals and L2 Learners. , 2004 .

[14]  Aditya Murthy,et al.  Covert inhibition potentiates online control in a double-step task. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[15]  F. Craik,et al.  Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[16]  Anat Prior Too much of a good thing: Stronger bilingual inhibition leads to larger lag-2 task repetition costs , 2012, Cognition.

[17]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  Bilingualism and the increased attentional blink effect: evidence that the difference between bilinguals and monolinguals generalizes to different levels of second language proficiency , 2013, Psychological research.

[18]  M. Carreiras,et al.  The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited: myth or reality? , 2014, Experimental psychology.

[19]  A. Vivas,et al.  Revisiting the bilingual advantage in attention in low SES Greek-Albanians: does the level of bilingual experience matter? , 2017 .

[20]  T. Egner Congruency sequence effects and cognitive control , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[21]  J. Altarriba,et al.  The relationship between language proficiency and attentional control in Cantonese-English bilingual children: evidence from Simon, Simon switching, and working memory tasks , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[22]  J. Hoffman,et al.  The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[23]  R. Mishra,et al.  Effect of bilingualism on anticipatory oculomotor control , 2016 .

[24]  D. Green,et al.  Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis , 2013, Journal of cognitive psychology.

[25]  E. Coderre,et al.  Electrophysiological Explorations of the Bilingual Advantage: Evidence from a Stroop Task , 2014, PloS one.

[26]  K. Fujii,et al.  Visualization for the analysis of fluid motion , 2005, J. Vis..

[27]  Kristin Lemhöfer,et al.  Introducing LexTALE: A quick and valid Lexical Test for Advanced Learners of English , 2011, Behavior research methods.

[28]  Wilkin Chau,et al.  Effect of bilingualism on cognitive control in the Simon task: evidence from MEG , 2005, NeuroImage.

[29]  D. Davidson,et al.  Is there a bilingual advantage in the ANT task? Evidence from children , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[30]  Zhilong Xie Second-Language Proficiency, Language Use, and Mental Set Shifting in Cognitive Control Among Unbalanced Chinese–English Bilinguals , 2014 .

[31]  Albert Costa,et al.  Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task , 2008, Cognition.

[32]  F. Craik,et al.  Executive control in a modified antisaccade task: Effects of aging and bilingualism. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[33]  K. Paap,et al.  Bilingual advantages in executive functioning: problems in convergent validity, discriminant validity, and the identification of the theoretical constructs , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[34]  E. Bialystok,et al.  Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: evidence from the dimensional change card sort task. , 2004, Developmental science.

[35]  E. Bialystok,et al.  Is there a relation between onset age of bilingualism and enhancement of cognitive control?* , 2011, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.

[36]  John W. Schwieter The Cambridge handbook of bilingual processing , 2015 .

[37]  R. Mishra,et al.  The modulatory role of second language proficiency on performance monitoring: evidence from a saccadic countermanding task in high and low proficient bilinguals , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[38]  M. Mon-Williams,et al.  Cognitive Control of Saccadic Eye Movements in Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder , 2016, PloS one.

[39]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[40]  J. Kroll,et al.  Understanding the consequences of bilingualism for language processing and cognition , 2013, Journal of cognitive psychology.

[41]  M. Bajo,et al.  Dual mechanisms of cognitive control in bilinguals and monolinguals , 2013 .

[42]  D. Green,et al.  Lemma selection without inhibition of languages in bilingual speakers , 1998, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.

[43]  Albert Costa,et al.  On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t , 2009, Cognition.

[44]  D. Titone,et al.  Inhibitory Control and L2 Proficiency Modulate Bilingual Language Production: Evidence from Spontaneous Monologue and Dialogue Speech , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[45]  F. Craik,et al.  Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: evidence from the Simon task. , 2004, Psychology and aging.

[46]  Tanya Dash,et al.  Bilingual Language Control and General Purpose Cognitive Control among Individuals with Bilingual Aphasia: Evidence Based on Negative Priming and Flanker Tasks , 2014, Behavioural neurology.

[47]  Shirley-Ann Rüschemeyer,et al.  A mouse with a roof? effects of phonological neighbors on processing of words in sentences in a non-native language , 2008, Brain and Language.

[48]  M. Bajo,et al.  Bilingualism modulates dual mechanisms of cognitive control: Evidence from ERPs , 2015, Neuropsychologia.

[49]  S. Ben Hamed,et al.  Proactive Inhibitory Control of Response as the Default State of Executive Control , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[50]  J. Velay,et al.  Cueing method biases in visual detection studies , 2007, Brain Research.

[51]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Neurolinguistics: Structural plasticity in the bilingual brain , 2004, Nature.

[52]  A. Krott,et al.  Bilingualism enhances attentional control in non-verbal conflict tasks – evidence from ex-Gaussian analyses* , 2016, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.

[53]  R. Mishra,et al.  Second language proficiency modulates conflict-monitoring in an oculomotor Stroop task: evidence from Hindi-English bilinguals , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[54]  T. Braver The variable nature of cognitive control: a dual mechanisms framework , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[55]  Philippe Boulinguez,et al.  Proactive inhibitory control of movement assessed by event-related fMRI , 2008, NeuroImage.

[56]  Alessandra S Souza,et al.  No evidence for bilingual cognitive advantages: A test of four hypotheses. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[57]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions , 1978, Vision Research.

[58]  S. Nieuwenhuis,et al.  How does bilingualism improve executive control? A comparison of active and reactive inhibition mechanisms. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[59]  D. Green Language Control in Different Contexts: The Behavioral Ecology of Bilingual Speakers , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[60]  K. Paap,et al.  There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing , 2013, Cognitive Psychology.

[61]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[62]  Antoni Rodríguez-Fornells,et al.  Executive Control in Bilingual Language Processing , 2006, Language Learning.