Effect of language proficiency on proactive occulo-motor control among bilinguals
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Ellen Bialystok,et al. Executive processes in appearance-reality tasks: the role of inhibition of attention and symbolic representation. , 2004, Child development.
[2] Virginia Valian. Bilingualism and cognition , 2014, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.
[3] J. Altarriba,et al. The effects of first- and second-language proficiency on conflict resolution and goal maintenance in bilinguals: Evidence from reaction time distributional analyses in a Stroop task* , 2012, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.
[4] Matthew D. Hilchey,et al. Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[5] A. Vivas,et al. Attentional processes in low-socioeconomic status bilingual children: are they modulated by the amount of bilingual experience? , 2015, Child development.
[6] E. Miller,et al. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.
[7] T. Bak,et al. Bilingualism delays age at onset of dementia, independent of education and immigration status , 2013, Neurology.
[8] John Ashburner,et al. Structural plasticity in the bilingual brain: Proficiency in a second language and age at acquisition affect grey-matter density. , 2004 .
[9] Philippe Boulinguez,et al. Warning signals induce automatic EMG activations and proactive volitional inhibition: evidence from analysis of error distribution in simple RT. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.
[10] A. Murthy,et al. Programming of double-step saccade sequences: Modulation by cognitive control , 2004, Vision Research.
[11] R. Mishra,et al. Does Language Proficiency Modulate Oculomotor Control? Evidence from Hindi-English Bilinguals. , 2012 .
[12] A. Ardila,et al. The effect of language proficiency on executive functions in balanced and unbalanced Spanish–English bilinguals * , 2016 .
[13] Albert Costa,et al. Lexical Access in Bilingual Speech Production: Evidence from Language Switching in Highly Proficient Bilinguals and L2 Learners. , 2004 .
[14] Aditya Murthy,et al. Covert inhibition potentiates online control in a double-step task. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[15] F. Craik,et al. Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[16] Anat Prior. Too much of a good thing: Stronger bilingual inhibition leads to larger lag-2 task repetition costs , 2012, Cognition.
[17] Marc Brysbaert,et al. Bilingualism and the increased attentional blink effect: evidence that the difference between bilinguals and monolinguals generalizes to different levels of second language proficiency , 2013, Psychological research.
[18] M. Carreiras,et al. The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited: myth or reality? , 2014, Experimental psychology.
[19] A. Vivas,et al. Revisiting the bilingual advantage in attention in low SES Greek-Albanians: does the level of bilingual experience matter? , 2017 .
[20] T. Egner. Congruency sequence effects and cognitive control , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[21] J. Altarriba,et al. The relationship between language proficiency and attentional control in Cantonese-English bilingual children: evidence from Simon, Simon switching, and working memory tasks , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[22] J. Hoffman,et al. The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.
[23] R. Mishra,et al. Effect of bilingualism on anticipatory oculomotor control , 2016 .
[24] D. Green,et al. Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis , 2013, Journal of cognitive psychology.
[25] E. Coderre,et al. Electrophysiological Explorations of the Bilingual Advantage: Evidence from a Stroop Task , 2014, PloS one.
[26] K. Fujii,et al. Visualization for the analysis of fluid motion , 2005, J. Vis..
[27] Kristin Lemhöfer,et al. Introducing LexTALE: A quick and valid Lexical Test for Advanced Learners of English , 2011, Behavior research methods.
[28] Wilkin Chau,et al. Effect of bilingualism on cognitive control in the Simon task: evidence from MEG , 2005, NeuroImage.
[29] D. Davidson,et al. Is there a bilingual advantage in the ANT task? Evidence from children , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[30] Zhilong Xie. Second-Language Proficiency, Language Use, and Mental Set Shifting in Cognitive Control Among Unbalanced Chinese–English Bilinguals , 2014 .
[31] Albert Costa,et al. Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task , 2008, Cognition.
[32] F. Craik,et al. Executive control in a modified antisaccade task: Effects of aging and bilingualism. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[33] K. Paap,et al. Bilingual advantages in executive functioning: problems in convergent validity, discriminant validity, and the identification of the theoretical constructs , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[34] E. Bialystok,et al. Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: evidence from the dimensional change card sort task. , 2004, Developmental science.
[35] E. Bialystok,et al. Is there a relation between onset age of bilingualism and enhancement of cognitive control?* , 2011, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.
[36] John W. Schwieter. The Cambridge handbook of bilingual processing , 2015 .
[37] R. Mishra,et al. The modulatory role of second language proficiency on performance monitoring: evidence from a saccadic countermanding task in high and low proficient bilinguals , 2015, Front. Psychol..
[38] M. Mon-Williams,et al. Cognitive Control of Saccadic Eye Movements in Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder , 2016, PloS one.
[39] J. G. Snodgrass,et al. A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.
[40] J. Kroll,et al. Understanding the consequences of bilingualism for language processing and cognition , 2013, Journal of cognitive psychology.
[41] M. Bajo,et al. Dual mechanisms of cognitive control in bilinguals and monolinguals , 2013 .
[42] D. Green,et al. Lemma selection without inhibition of languages in bilingual speakers , 1998, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.
[43] Albert Costa,et al. On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t , 2009, Cognition.
[44] D. Titone,et al. Inhibitory Control and L2 Proficiency Modulate Bilingual Language Production: Evidence from Spontaneous Monologue and Dialogue Speech , 2012, Front. Psychology.
[45] F. Craik,et al. Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: evidence from the Simon task. , 2004, Psychology and aging.
[46] Tanya Dash,et al. Bilingual Language Control and General Purpose Cognitive Control among Individuals with Bilingual Aphasia: Evidence Based on Negative Priming and Flanker Tasks , 2014, Behavioural neurology.
[47] Shirley-Ann Rüschemeyer,et al. A mouse with a roof? effects of phonological neighbors on processing of words in sentences in a non-native language , 2008, Brain and Language.
[48] M. Bajo,et al. Bilingualism modulates dual mechanisms of cognitive control: Evidence from ERPs , 2015, Neuropsychologia.
[49] S. Ben Hamed,et al. Proactive Inhibitory Control of Response as the Default State of Executive Control , 2012, Front. Psychology.
[50] J. Velay,et al. Cueing method biases in visual detection studies , 2007, Brain Research.
[51] Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al. Neurolinguistics: Structural plasticity in the bilingual brain , 2004, Nature.
[52] A. Krott,et al. Bilingualism enhances attentional control in non-verbal conflict tasks – evidence from ex-Gaussian analyses* , 2016, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.
[53] R. Mishra,et al. Second language proficiency modulates conflict-monitoring in an oculomotor Stroop task: evidence from Hindi-English bilinguals , 2013, Front. Psychol..
[54] T. Braver. The variable nature of cognitive control: a dual mechanisms framework , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[55] Philippe Boulinguez,et al. Proactive inhibitory control of movement assessed by event-related fMRI , 2008, NeuroImage.
[56] Alessandra S Souza,et al. No evidence for bilingual cognitive advantages: A test of four hypotheses. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[57] P. E. Hallett,et al. Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions , 1978, Vision Research.
[58] S. Nieuwenhuis,et al. How does bilingualism improve executive control? A comparison of active and reactive inhibition mechanisms. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[59] D. Green. Language Control in Different Contexts: The Behavioral Ecology of Bilingual Speakers , 2011, Front. Psychology.
[60] K. Paap,et al. There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing , 2013, Cognitive Psychology.
[61] M. Botvinick,et al. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.
[62] Antoni Rodríguez-Fornells,et al. Executive Control in Bilingual Language Processing , 2006, Language Learning.